The optimism bias
-
0:00 - 0:04I'm going to talk to you about optimism --
-
0:04 - 0:06or more precisely, the optimism bias.
-
0:06 - 0:08It's a cognitive illusion
-
0:08 - 0:10that we've been studying in my lab for the past few years,
-
0:10 - 0:12and 80 percent of us have it.
-
0:12 - 0:15It's our tendency to overestimate
-
0:15 - 0:18our likelihood of experiencing good events in our lives
-
0:18 - 0:22and underestimate our likelihood of experiencing bad events.
-
0:22 - 0:25So we underestimate our likelihood of suffering from cancer,
-
0:25 - 0:26being in a car accident.
-
0:26 - 0:30We overestimate our longevity, our career prospects.
-
0:30 - 0:33In short, we're more optimistic than realistic,
-
0:33 - 0:35but we are oblivious to the fact.
-
0:35 - 0:37Take marriage for example.
-
0:37 - 0:41In the Western world, divorce rates are about 40 percent.
-
0:41 - 0:44That means that out of five married couples,
-
0:44 - 0:47two will end up splitting their assets.
-
0:47 - 0:51But when you ask newlyweds about their own likelihood of divorce,
-
0:51 - 0:54they estimate it at zero percent.
-
0:54 - 0:58And even divorce lawyers, who should really know better,
-
0:58 - 1:02hugely underestimate their own likelihood of divorce.
-
1:02 - 1:05So it turns out that optimists are not less likely to divorce,
-
1:05 - 1:07but they are more likely to remarry.
-
1:07 - 1:10In the words of Samuel Johnson,
-
1:10 - 1:14"Remarriage is the triumph of hope over experience."
-
1:14 - 1:16(Laughter)
-
1:16 - 1:20So if we're married, we're more likely to have kids.
-
1:20 - 1:24And we all think our kids will be especially talented.
-
1:24 - 1:26This, by the way, is my two-year-old nephew, Guy.
-
1:26 - 1:29And I just want to make it absolutely clear
-
1:29 - 1:31that he's a really bad example of the optimism bias,
-
1:31 - 1:34because he is in fact uniquely talented.
-
1:34 - 1:36(Laughter)
-
1:36 - 1:37And I'm not alone.
-
1:37 - 1:40Out of four British people, three said
-
1:40 - 1:43that they were optimistic about the future of their own families.
-
1:43 - 1:45That's 75 percent.
-
1:45 - 1:47But only 30 percent said
-
1:47 - 1:50that they thought families in general
-
1:50 - 1:52are doing better than a few generations ago.
-
1:52 - 1:54And this is a really important point,
-
1:54 - 1:56because we're optimistic about ourselves,
-
1:56 - 1:58we're optimistic about our kids,
-
1:58 - 2:00we're optimistic about our families,
-
2:00 - 2:03but we're not so optimistic about the guy sitting next to us,
-
2:03 - 2:05and we're somewhat pessimistic
-
2:05 - 2:09about the fate of our fellow citizens and the fate of our country.
-
2:09 - 2:13But private optimism about our own personal future
-
2:13 - 2:15remains persistent.
-
2:15 - 2:19And it doesn't mean that we think things will magically turn out okay,
-
2:19 - 2:23but rather that we have the unique ability to make it so.
-
2:23 - 2:26Now I'm a scientist, I do experiments.
-
2:26 - 2:28So to show you what I mean,
-
2:28 - 2:31I'm going to do an experiment here with you.
-
2:31 - 2:34So I'm going to give you a list of abilities and characteristics,
-
2:34 - 2:37and I want you to think for each of these abilities
-
2:37 - 2:42where you stand relative to the rest of the population.
-
2:42 - 2:45The first one is getting along well with others.
-
2:45 - 2:51Who here believes they're at the bottom 25 percent?
-
2:51 - 2:55Okay, that's about 10 people out of 1,500.
-
2:55 - 2:59Who believes they're at the top 25 percent?
-
2:59 - 3:02That's most of us here.
-
3:02 - 3:07Okay, now do the same for your driving ability.
-
3:07 - 3:10How interesting are you?
-
3:10 - 3:13How attractive are you?
-
3:13 - 3:15How honest are you?
-
3:15 - 3:20And finally, how modest are you?
-
3:20 - 3:23So most of us put ourselves above average
-
3:23 - 3:25on most of these abilities.
-
3:25 - 3:27Now this is statistically impossible.
-
3:27 - 3:31We can't all be better than everyone else.
-
3:31 - 3:32(Laughter)
-
3:32 - 3:35But if we believe we're better than the other guy,
-
3:35 - 3:39well that means that we're more likely to get that promotion, to remain married,
-
3:39 - 3:42because we're more social, more interesting.
-
3:42 - 3:44And it's a global phenomenon.
-
3:44 - 3:46The optimism bias has been observed
-
3:46 - 3:48in many different countries --
-
3:48 - 3:51in Western cultures, in non-Western cultures,
-
3:51 - 3:53in females and males,
-
3:53 - 3:54in kids, in the elderly.
-
3:54 - 3:56It's quite widespread.
-
3:56 - 4:00But the question is, is it good for us?
-
4:00 - 4:02So some people say no.
-
4:02 - 4:04Some people say the secret to happiness
-
4:04 - 4:07is low expectations.
-
4:07 - 4:10I think the logic goes something like this:
-
4:10 - 4:12If we don't expect greatness,
-
4:12 - 4:16if we don't expect to find love and be healthy and successful,
-
4:16 - 4:19well we're not going to be disappointed when these things don't happen.
-
4:19 - 4:22And if we're not disappointed when good things don't happen,
-
4:22 - 4:24and we're pleasantly surprised when they do,
-
4:24 - 4:26we will be happy.
-
4:26 - 4:28So it's a very good theory,
-
4:28 - 4:31but it turns out to be wrong for three reasons.
-
4:31 - 4:36Number one: Whatever happens, whether you succeed or you fail,
-
4:36 - 4:39people with high expectations always feel better.
-
4:39 - 4:43Because how we feel when we get dumped or win employee of the month
-
4:43 - 4:46depends on how we interpret that event.
-
4:46 - 4:50The psychologists Margaret Marshall and John Brown
-
4:50 - 4:53studied students with high and low expectations.
-
4:53 - 4:58And they found that when people with high expectations succeed,
-
4:58 - 5:00they attribute that success to their own traits.
-
5:00 - 5:03"I'm a genius, therefore I got an A,
-
5:03 - 5:05therefore I'll get an A again and again in the future."
-
5:05 - 5:08When they failed, it wasn't because they were dumb,
-
5:08 - 5:11but because the exam just happened to be unfair.
-
5:11 - 5:14Next time they will do better.
-
5:14 - 5:17People with low expectations do the opposite.
-
5:17 - 5:20So when they failed it was because they were dumb,
-
5:20 - 5:21and when they succeeded
-
5:21 - 5:24it was because the exam just happened to be really easy.
-
5:24 - 5:27Next time reality would catch up with them.
-
5:27 - 5:29So they felt worse.
-
5:29 - 5:32Number two: Regardless of the outcome,
-
5:32 - 5:36the pure act of anticipation makes us happy.
-
5:36 - 5:39The behavioral economist George Lowenstein
-
5:39 - 5:41asked students in his university
-
5:41 - 5:46to imagine getting a passionate kiss from a celebrity, any celebrity.
-
5:46 - 5:48Then he said, "How much are you willing to pay
-
5:48 - 5:50to get a kiss from a celebrity
-
5:50 - 5:53if the kiss was delivered immediately,
-
5:53 - 5:58in three hours, in 24 hours, in three days,
-
5:58 - 6:00in one year, in 10 years?
-
6:00 - 6:03He found that the students were willing to pay the most
-
6:03 - 6:05not to get a kiss immediately,
-
6:05 - 6:08but to get a kiss in three days.
-
6:08 - 6:12They were willing to pay extra in order to wait.
-
6:12 - 6:15Now they weren't willing to wait a year or 10 years;
-
6:15 - 6:17no one wants an aging celebrity.
-
6:17 - 6:22But three days seemed to be the optimum amount.
-
6:22 - 6:24So why is that?
-
6:24 - 6:27Well if you get the kiss now, it's over and done with.
-
6:27 - 6:29But if you get the kiss in three days,
-
6:29 - 6:33well that's three days of jittery anticipation, the thrill of the wait.
-
6:33 - 6:35The students wanted that time
-
6:35 - 6:38to imagine where is it going to happen,
-
6:38 - 6:39how is it going to happen.
-
6:39 - 6:42Anticipation made them happy.
-
6:42 - 6:45This is, by the way, why people prefer Friday to Sunday.
-
6:45 - 6:48It's a really curious fact,
-
6:48 - 6:51because Friday is a day of work and Sunday is a day of pleasure,
-
6:51 - 6:54so you'd assume that people will prefer Sunday,
-
6:54 - 6:56but they don't.
-
6:56 - 6:58It's not because they really, really like being in the office
-
6:58 - 7:00and they can't stand strolling in the park
-
7:00 - 7:02or having a lazy brunch.
-
7:02 - 7:04We know that, because when you ask people
-
7:04 - 7:07about their ultimate favorite day of the week,
-
7:07 - 7:10surprise, surprise, Saturday comes in at first,
-
7:10 - 7:13then Friday, then Sunday.
-
7:13 - 7:14People prefer Friday
-
7:14 - 7:18because Friday brings with it the anticipation of the weekend ahead,
-
7:18 - 7:20all the plans that you have.
-
7:20 - 7:23On Sunday, the only thing you can look forward to
-
7:23 - 7:25is the work week.
-
7:25 - 7:30So optimists are people who expect more kisses in their future,
-
7:30 - 7:32more strolls in the park.
-
7:32 - 7:36And that anticipation enhances their wellbeing.
-
7:36 - 7:39In fact, without the optimism bias,
-
7:39 - 7:42we would all be slightly depressed.
-
7:42 - 7:44People with mild depression,
-
7:44 - 7:47they don't have a bias when they look into the future.
-
7:47 - 7:51They're actually more realistic than healthy individuals.
-
7:51 - 7:53But individuals with severe depression,
-
7:53 - 7:55they have a pessimistic bias.
-
7:55 - 7:58So they tend to expect the future
-
7:58 - 8:00to be worse than it ends up being.
-
8:00 - 8:03So optimism changes subjective reality.
-
8:03 - 8:07The way we expect the world to be changes the way we see it.
-
8:07 - 8:10But it also changes objective reality.
-
8:10 - 8:13It acts as a self-fulfilling prophecy.
-
8:13 - 8:15And that is the third reason
-
8:15 - 8:18why lowering your expectations will not make you happy.
-
8:18 - 8:20Controlled experiments have shown
-
8:20 - 8:23that optimism is not only related to success,
-
8:23 - 8:25it leads to success.
-
8:25 - 8:30Optimism leads to success in academia and sports and politics.
-
8:30 - 8:34And maybe the most surprising benefit of optimism is health.
-
8:34 - 8:38If we expect the future to be bright,
-
8:38 - 8:40stress and anxiety are reduced.
-
8:40 - 8:44So all in all, optimism has lots of benefits.
-
8:44 - 8:48But the question that was really confusing to me was,
-
8:48 - 8:52how do we maintain optimism in the face of reality?
-
8:52 - 8:55As an neuroscientist, this was especially confusing,
-
8:55 - 8:58because according to all the theories out there,
-
8:58 - 9:02when your expectations are not met, you should alter them.
-
9:02 - 9:04But this is not what we find.
-
9:04 - 9:07We asked people to come into our lab
-
9:07 - 9:10in order to try and figure out what was going on.
-
9:10 - 9:13We asked them to estimate their likelihood
-
9:13 - 9:15of experiencing different terrible events in their lives.
-
9:15 - 9:20So, for example, what is your likelihood of suffering from cancer?
-
9:20 - 9:22And then we told them the average likelihood
-
9:22 - 9:25of someone like them to suffer these misfortunes.
-
9:25 - 9:28So cancer, for example, is about 30 percent.
-
9:28 - 9:31And then we asked them again,
-
9:31 - 9:34"How likely are you to suffer from cancer?"
-
9:34 - 9:36What we wanted to know was
-
9:36 - 9:39whether people will take the information that we gave them
-
9:39 - 9:41to change their beliefs.
-
9:41 - 9:44And indeed they did --
-
9:44 - 9:46but mostly when the information we gave them
-
9:46 - 9:49was better than what they expected.
-
9:49 - 9:50So for example,
-
9:50 - 9:53if someone said, "My likelihood of suffering from cancer
-
9:53 - 9:56is about 50 percent,"
-
9:56 - 9:58and we said, "Hey, good news.
-
9:58 - 10:01The average likelihood is only 30 percent,"
-
10:01 - 10:03the next time around they would say,
-
10:03 - 10:06"Well maybe my likelihood is about 35 percent."
-
10:06 - 10:08So they learned quickly and efficiently.
-
10:08 - 10:11But if someone started off saying,
-
10:11 - 10:14"My average likelihood of suffering from cancer is about 10 percent,"
-
10:14 - 10:17and we said, "Hey, bad news.
-
10:17 - 10:20The average likelihood is about 30 percent,"
-
10:20 - 10:22the next time around they would say,
-
10:22 - 10:25"Yep. Still think it's about 11 percent."
-
10:25 - 10:27(Laughter)
-
10:27 - 10:30So it's not that they didn't learn at all -- they did --
-
10:30 - 10:32but much, much less than when we gave them
-
10:32 - 10:35positive information about the future.
-
10:35 - 10:38And it's not that they didn't remember the numbers that we gave them;
-
10:38 - 10:41everyone remembers that the average likelihood of cancer
-
10:41 - 10:43is about 30 percent
-
10:43 - 10:45and the average likelihood of divorce is about 40 percent.
-
10:45 - 10:50But they didn't think that those numbers were related to them.
-
10:50 - 10:54What this means is that warning signs such as these
-
10:54 - 10:57may only have limited impact.
-
10:57 - 11:01Yes, smoking kills, but mostly it kills the other guy.
-
11:01 - 11:03What I wanted to know was
-
11:03 - 11:06what was going on inside the human brain
-
11:06 - 11:10that prevented us from taking these warning signs personally.
-
11:10 - 11:11But at the same time,
-
11:11 - 11:13when we hear that the housing market is hopeful,
-
11:13 - 11:18we think, "Oh, my house is definitely going to double in price."
-
11:18 - 11:20To try and figure that out,
-
11:20 - 11:22I asked the participants in the experiment
-
11:22 - 11:24to lie in a brain imaging scanner.
-
11:24 - 11:26It looks like this.
-
11:26 - 11:29And using a method called functional MRI,
-
11:29 - 11:32we were able to identify regions in the brain
-
11:32 - 11:35that were responding to positive information.
-
11:35 - 11:39One of these regions is called the left inferior frontal gyrus.
-
11:39 - 11:43So if someone said, "My likelihood of suffering from cancer is 50 percent,"
-
11:43 - 11:44and we said, "Hey, good news.
-
11:44 - 11:47Average likelihood is 30 percent,"
-
11:47 - 11:50the left inferior frontal gyrus would respond fiercely.
-
11:50 - 11:55And it didn't matter if you're an extreme optimist, a mild optimist
-
11:55 - 11:57or slightly pessimistic,
-
11:57 - 12:00everyone's left inferior frontal gyrus
-
12:00 - 12:01was functioning perfectly well,
-
12:01 - 12:04whether you're Barack Obama or Woody Allen.
-
12:04 - 12:06On the other side of the brain,
-
12:06 - 12:11the right inferior frontal gyrus was responding to bad news.
-
12:11 - 12:14And here's the thing: it wasn't doing a very good job.
-
12:14 - 12:16The more optimistic you were,
-
12:16 - 12:19the less likely this region was
-
12:19 - 12:22to respond to unexpected negative information.
-
12:22 - 12:25And if your brain is failing
-
12:25 - 12:28at integrating bad news about the future,
-
12:28 - 12:33you will constantly leave your rose-tinted spectacles on.
-
12:33 - 12:38So we wanted to know, could we change this?
-
12:38 - 12:41Could we alter people's optimism bias
-
12:41 - 12:45by interfering with the brain activity in these regions?
-
12:45 - 12:48And there's a way for us to do that.
-
12:48 - 12:50This is my collaborator Ryota Kanai.
-
12:50 - 12:54And what he's doing is he's passing a small magnetic pulse
-
12:54 - 12:56through the skull of the participant in our study
-
12:56 - 12:59into their inferior frontal gyrus.
-
12:59 - 13:00And by doing that,
-
13:00 - 13:03he's interfering with the activity of this brain region
-
13:03 - 13:05for about half an hour.
-
13:05 - 13:07After that everything goes back to normal, I assure you.
-
13:07 - 13:09(Laughter)
-
13:09 - 13:13So let's see what happens.
-
13:13 - 13:15First of all, I'm going to show you
-
13:15 - 13:17the average amount of bias that we see.
-
13:17 - 13:20So if I was to test all of you now,
-
13:20 - 13:22this is the amount that you would learn
-
13:22 - 13:25more from good news relative to bad news.
-
13:25 - 13:28Now we interfere with the region
-
13:28 - 13:32that we found to integrate negative information in this task,
-
13:32 - 13:36and the optimism bias grew even larger.
-
13:36 - 13:41We made people more biased in the way that they process information.
-
13:41 - 13:44Then we interfered with the brain region
-
13:44 - 13:48that we found to integrate good news in this task,
-
13:48 - 13:52and the optimism bias disappeared.
-
13:52 - 13:54We were quite amazed by these results
-
13:54 - 13:56because we were able to eliminate
-
13:56 - 13:59a deep-rooted bias in humans.
-
13:59 - 14:04And at this point we stopped and we asked ourselves,
-
14:04 - 14:09would we want to shatter the optimism illusion into tiny little bits?
-
14:09 - 14:14If we could do that, would we want to take people's optimism bias away?
-
14:14 - 14:19Well I've already told you about all of the benefits of the optimism bias,
-
14:19 - 14:23which probably makes you want to hold onto it for dear life.
-
14:23 - 14:25But there are, of course, pitfalls,
-
14:25 - 14:28and it would be really foolish of us to ignore them.
-
14:28 - 14:32Take for example this email I recieved
-
14:32 - 14:35from a firefighter here in California.
-
14:35 - 14:38He says, "Fatality investigations for firefighters
-
14:38 - 14:42often include 'We didn't think the fire was going to do that,'
-
14:42 - 14:44even when all of the available information
-
14:44 - 14:47was there to make safe decisions."
-
14:47 - 14:51This captain is going to use our findings on the optimism bias
-
14:51 - 14:53to try to explain to the firefighters
-
14:53 - 14:55why they think the way they do,
-
14:55 - 15:02to make them acutely aware of this very optimistic bias in humans.
-
15:02 - 15:07So unrealistic optimism can lead to risky behavior,
-
15:07 - 15:11to financial collapse, to faulty planning.
-
15:11 - 15:13The British government, for example,
-
15:13 - 15:16has acknowledged that the optimism bias
-
15:16 - 15:19can make individuals more likely
-
15:19 - 15:23to underestimate the costs and durations of projects.
-
15:23 - 15:27So they have adjusted the 2012 Olympic budget
-
15:27 - 15:29for the optimism bias.
-
15:29 - 15:32My friend who's getting married in a few weeks
-
15:32 - 15:34has done the same for his wedding budget.
-
15:34 - 15:37And by the way, when I asked him about his own likelihood of divorce,
-
15:37 - 15:41he said he was quite sure it was zero percent.
-
15:41 - 15:43So what we would really like to do,
-
15:43 - 15:47is we would like to protect ourselves from the dangers of optimism,
-
15:47 - 15:50but at the same time remain hopeful,
-
15:50 - 15:53benefiting from the many fruits of optimism.
-
15:53 - 15:56And I believe there's a way for us to do that.
-
15:56 - 15:58The key here really is knowledge.
-
15:58 - 16:01We're not born with an innate understanding of our biases.
-
16:01 - 16:05These have to be identified by scientific investigation.
-
16:05 - 16:09But the good news is that becoming aware of the optimism bias
-
16:09 - 16:11does not shatter the illusion.
-
16:11 - 16:13It's like visual illusions,
-
16:13 - 16:16in which understanding them does not make them go away.
-
16:16 - 16:19And this is good because it means
-
16:19 - 16:21we should be able to strike a balance,
-
16:21 - 16:23to come up with plans and rules
-
16:23 - 16:26to protect ourselves from unrealistic optimism,
-
16:26 - 16:29but at the same time remain hopeful.
-
16:29 - 16:33I think this cartoon portrays it nicely.
-
16:33 - 16:36Because if you're one of these pessimistic penguins up there
-
16:36 - 16:38who just does not believe they can fly,
-
16:38 - 16:41you certainly never will.
-
16:41 - 16:43Because to make any kind of progress,
-
16:43 - 16:45we need to be able to imagine a different reality,
-
16:45 - 16:49and then we need to believe that that reality is possible.
-
16:49 - 16:52But if you are an extreme optimistic penguin
-
16:52 - 16:55who just jumps down blindly hoping for the best,
-
16:55 - 17:00you might find yourself in a bit of a mess when you hit the ground.
-
17:00 - 17:02But if you're an optimistic penguin
-
17:02 - 17:03who believes they can fly,
-
17:03 - 17:06but then adjusts a parachute to your back
-
17:06 - 17:09just in case things don't work out exactly as you had planned,
-
17:09 - 17:11you will soar like an eagle,
-
17:11 - 17:14even if you're just a penguin.
-
17:14 - 17:16Thank you.
-
17:16 - 17:19(Applause)
- Title:
- The optimism bias
- Speaker:
- Tali Sharot
- Description:
-
Are we born to be optimistic, rather than realistic? Tali Sharot shares new research that suggests our brains are wired to look on the bright side -- and how that can be both dangerous and beneficial.
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
- closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 17:40
Jenny Zurawell edited English subtitles for The optimism bias | ||
Jenny Zurawell approved English subtitles for The optimism bias | ||
Jenny Zurawell edited English subtitles for The optimism bias | ||
Morton Bast accepted English subtitles for The optimism bias | ||
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for The optimism bias | ||
Timothy Covell added a translation |