< Return to Video

A Darwinian theory of beauty

  • 0:00 - 0:03
    Delighted to be here
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    and to talk to you about a subject dear to my heart,
  • 0:05 - 0:08
    which is beauty.
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    I do the philosophy of art, aesthetics,
  • 0:11 - 0:13
    actually, for a living.
  • 0:13 - 0:15
    I try to figure out intellectually,
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    philosophically, psychologically,
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    what the experience of beauty is,
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    what sensibly can be said about it
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    and how people go off the rails in trying to understand it.
  • 0:26 - 0:29
    Now this is an extremely complicated subject,
  • 0:29 - 0:32
    in part because the things that we call beautiful
  • 0:32 - 0:34
    are so different.
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    I mean just think of the sheer variety --
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    a baby's face,
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    Berlioz's "Harold in Italy,"
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    movies like "The Wizard of Oz"
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    or the plays of Chekhov,
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    a central California landscape,
  • 0:46 - 0:49
    a Hokusai view of Mt. Fuji,
  • 0:49 - 0:51
    "Der Rosenkavalier,"
  • 0:51 - 0:53
    a stunning match-winning goal
  • 0:53 - 0:55
    in a World Cup soccer match,
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    Van Gogh's "Starry Night,"
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    a Jane Austen novel,
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    Fred Astaire dancing across the screen.
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    This brief list includes human beings,
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    natural landforms,
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    works of art and skilled human actions.
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    An account that explains the presence of beauty
  • 1:13 - 1:15
    in everything on this list
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    is not going to be easy.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    I can, however, give you at least a taste
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    of what I regard
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    as the most powerful theory of beauty
  • 1:24 - 1:26
    we yet have.
  • 1:26 - 1:28
    And we get it not from a philosopher of art,
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    not from a postmodern art theorist
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    or a bigwig art critic.
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    No, this theory
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    comes from an expert
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    on barnacles and worms and pigeon breeding,
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    and you know who I mean:
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    Charles Darwin.
  • 1:47 - 1:50
    Of course, a lot of people think they already know
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    the proper answer to the question,
  • 1:53 - 1:55
    "What is beauty?"
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    It's in the eye of the beholder.
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    It's whatever moves you personally.
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    Or, as some people,
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    especially academics prefer,
  • 2:04 - 2:07
    beauty is in the culturally conditioned
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    eye of the beholder.
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    People agree that paintings or movies or music
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    are beautiful
  • 2:14 - 2:18
    because their cultures determine a uniformity of aesthetic taste.
  • 2:18 - 2:21
    Taste for both natural beauty and for the arts
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    travel across cultures
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    with great ease.
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    Beethoven is adored in Japan.
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    Peruvians love Japanese woodblock prints.
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    Inca sculptures are regarded as treasures
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    in British museums,
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    while Shakespeare is translated
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    into every major language of the Earth.
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    Or just think about American jazz
  • 2:41 - 2:43
    or American movies --
  • 2:43 - 2:45
    they go everywhere.
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    There are many differences among the arts,
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    but there are also universal,
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    cross-cultural aesthetic pleasures
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    and values.
  • 2:54 - 2:57
    How can we explain
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    this universality?
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    The best answer lies in trying to reconstruct
  • 3:02 - 3:05
    a Darwinian evolutionary history
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    of our artistic and aesthetic tastes.
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    We need to reverse-engineer
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    our present artistic tastes and preferences
  • 3:13 - 3:15
    and explain how they came
  • 3:15 - 3:18
    to be engraved in our minds
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    by the actions of both our prehistoric,
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    largely pleistocene environments,
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    where we became fully human,
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    but also by the social situations
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    in which we evolved.
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    This reverse engineering
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    can also enlist help
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    from the human record
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    preserved in prehistory.
  • 3:38 - 3:41
    I mean fossils, cave paintings and so forth.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    And it should take into account
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    what we know of the aesthetic interests
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    of isolated hunter-gatherer bands
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    that survived into the 19th and the 20th centuries.
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    Now, I personally
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    have no doubt whatsoever
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    that the experience of beauty,
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    with its emotional intensity and pleasure,
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    belongs to our evolved human psychology.
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    The experience of beauty is one component
  • 4:08 - 4:11
    in a whole series of Darwinian adaptations.
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    Beauty is an adaptive effect,
  • 4:14 - 4:16
    which we extend
  • 4:16 - 4:18
    and intensify
  • 4:18 - 4:20
    in the creation and enjoyment
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    of works of art and entertainment.
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    As many of you will know,
  • 4:26 - 4:29
    evolution operates by two main primary mechanisms.
  • 4:29 - 4:32
    The first of these is natural selection --
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    that's random mutation and selective retention --
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    along with our basic anatomy and physiology --
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    the evolution of the pancreas or the eye or the fingernails.
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    Natural selection also explains
  • 4:44 - 4:46
    many basic revulsions,
  • 4:46 - 4:48
    such as the horrid smell of rotting meat,
  • 4:48 - 4:51
    or fears, such as the fear of snakes
  • 4:51 - 4:54
    or standing close to the edge of a cliff.
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    Natural selection also explains pleasures --
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    sexual pleasure,
  • 4:59 - 5:02
    our liking for sweet, fat and proteins,
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    which in turn explains a lot of popular foods,
  • 5:05 - 5:08
    from ripe fruits through chocolate malts
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    and barbecued ribs.
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    The other great principle of evolution
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    is sexual selection,
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    and it operates very differently.
  • 5:17 - 5:20
    The peacock's magnificent tail
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    is the most famous example of this.
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    It did not evolve for natural survival.
  • 5:26 - 5:29
    In fact, it goes against natural survival.
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    No, the peacock's tail
  • 5:31 - 5:33
    results from the mating choices
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    made by peahens.
  • 5:35 - 5:37
    It's quite a familiar story.
  • 5:37 - 5:40
    It's women who actually push history forward.
  • 5:41 - 5:43
    Darwin himself, by the way,
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    had no doubts that the peacock's tail
  • 5:45 - 5:47
    was beautiful in the eyes of the peahen.
  • 5:47 - 5:50
    He actually used that word.
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    Now, keeping these ideas firmly in mind,
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    we can say that the experience of beauty
  • 5:56 - 5:59
    is one of the ways that evolution has
  • 5:59 - 6:01
    of arousing and sustaining
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    interest or fascination,
  • 6:03 - 6:05
    even obsession,
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    in order to encourage us
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    toward making the most adaptive decisions
  • 6:10 - 6:13
    for survival and reproduction.
  • 6:14 - 6:16
    Beauty is nature's way
  • 6:16 - 6:19
    of acting at a distance,
  • 6:19 - 6:21
    so to speak.
  • 6:21 - 6:23
    I mean, you can't expect to eat
  • 6:23 - 6:25
    an adaptively beneficial landscape.
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    It would hardly do to eat your baby
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    or your lover.
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    So evolution's trick
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    is to make them beautiful,
  • 6:33 - 6:36
    to have them exert a kind of magnetism
  • 6:36 - 6:39
    to give you the pleasure of simply looking at them.
  • 6:40 - 6:43
    Consider briefly an important source of aesthetic pleasure,
  • 6:43 - 6:45
    the magnetic pull
  • 6:45 - 6:47
    of beautiful landscapes.
  • 6:47 - 6:49
    People in very different cultures
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    all over the world
  • 6:51 - 6:54
    tend to like a particular kind of landscape,
  • 6:54 - 6:57
    a landscape that just happens to be similar
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    to the pleistocene savannas where we evolved.
  • 7:00 - 7:02
    This landscape shows up today
  • 7:02 - 7:05
    on calendars, on postcards,
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    in the design of golf courses and public parks
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    and in gold-framed pictures
  • 7:10 - 7:12
    that hang in living rooms
  • 7:12 - 7:15
    from New York to New Zealand.
  • 7:15 - 7:18
    It's a kind of Hudson River school landscape
  • 7:18 - 7:20
    featuring open spaces
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    of low grasses
  • 7:22 - 7:25
    interspersed with copses of trees.
  • 7:25 - 7:27
    The trees, by the way, are often preferred
  • 7:27 - 7:29
    if they fork near the ground,
  • 7:29 - 7:32
    that is to say, if they're trees you could scramble up
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    if you were in a tight fix.
  • 7:35 - 7:37
    The landscape shows the presence
  • 7:37 - 7:39
    of water directly in view,
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    or evidence of water in a bluish distance,
  • 7:43 - 7:46
    indications of animal or bird life
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    as well as diverse greenery
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    and finally -- get this --
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    a path
  • 7:53 - 7:55
    or a road,
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    perhaps a riverbank or a shoreline,
  • 7:58 - 8:01
    that extends into the distance,
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    almost inviting you to follow it.
  • 8:05 - 8:08
    This landscape type is regarded as beautiful,
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    even by people in countries
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    that don't have it.
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    The ideal savanna landscape
  • 8:14 - 8:16
    is one of the clearest examples
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    where human beings everywhere
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    find beauty
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    in similar visual experience.
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    But, someone might argue,
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    that's natural beauty.
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    How about artistic beauty?
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    Isn't that exhaustively cultural?
  • 8:32 - 8:34
    No, I don't think it is.
  • 8:34 - 8:37
    And once again, I'd like to look back to prehistory
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    to say something about it.
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    It is widely assumed
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    that the earliest human artworks
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    are the stupendously skillful cave paintings
  • 8:46 - 8:48
    that we all know from Lascaux
  • 8:48 - 8:50
    and Chauvet.
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    Chauvet caves
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    are about 32,000 years old,
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    along with a few small, realistic sculptures
  • 8:58 - 9:01
    of women and animals from the same period.
  • 9:05 - 9:07
    But artistic and decorative skills
  • 9:07 - 9:10
    are actually much older than that.
  • 9:11 - 9:13
    Beautiful shell necklaces
  • 9:13 - 9:16
    that look like something you'd see at an arts and crafts fair,
  • 9:16 - 9:18
    as well as ochre body paint,
  • 9:18 - 9:20
    have been found
  • 9:20 - 9:22
    from around 100,000 years ago.
  • 9:22 - 9:25
    But the most intriguing prehistoric artifacts
  • 9:25 - 9:27
    are older even than this.
  • 9:27 - 9:29
    I have in mind
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    the so-called Acheulian hand axes.
  • 9:33 - 9:36
    The oldest stone tools are choppers
  • 9:36 - 9:38
    from the Olduvai Gorge in East Africa.
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    They go back about two-and-a-half-million years.
  • 9:41 - 9:43
    These crude tools
  • 9:43 - 9:46
    were around for thousands of centuries,
  • 9:46 - 9:49
    until around 1.4 million years ago
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    when Homo erectus
  • 9:51 - 9:53
    started shaping
  • 9:53 - 9:55
    single, thin stone blades,
  • 9:55 - 9:58
    sometimes rounded ovals,
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    but often in what are to our eyes
  • 10:00 - 10:03
    an arresting, symmetrical pointed leaf
  • 10:03 - 10:05
    or teardrop form.
  • 10:05 - 10:07
    These Acheulian hand axes --
  • 10:07 - 10:09
    they're named after St. Acheul in France,
  • 10:09 - 10:12
    where finds were made in 19th century --
  • 10:12 - 10:15
    have been unearthed in their thousands,
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    scattered across Asia, Europe and Africa,
  • 10:18 - 10:21
    almost everywhere Homo erectus
  • 10:21 - 10:24
    and Homo ergaster roamed.
  • 10:24 - 10:27
    Now, the sheer numbers of these hand axes
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    shows that they can't have been made
  • 10:29 - 10:31
    for butchering animals.
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    And the plot really thickens when you realize
  • 10:34 - 10:37
    that, unlike other pleistocene tools,
  • 10:37 - 10:39
    the hand axes often exhibit
  • 10:39 - 10:41
    no evidence of wear
  • 10:41 - 10:43
    on their delicate blade edges.
  • 10:43 - 10:45
    And some, in any event, are too big
  • 10:45 - 10:47
    to use for butchery.
  • 10:48 - 10:50
    Their symmetry, their attractive materials
  • 10:50 - 10:52
    and, above all,
  • 10:52 - 10:54
    their meticulous workmanship
  • 10:54 - 10:57
    are simply quite beautiful
  • 10:57 - 11:00
    to our eyes, even today.
  • 11:00 - 11:03
    So what were these ancient --
  • 11:04 - 11:06
    I mean, they're ancient, they're foreign,
  • 11:06 - 11:08
    but they're at the same time
  • 11:08 - 11:10
    somehow familiar.
  • 11:10 - 11:13
    What were these artifacts for?
  • 11:13 - 11:15
    The best available answer
  • 11:15 - 11:17
    is that they were literally
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    the earliest known works of art,
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    practical tools transformed
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    into captivating aesthetic objects,
  • 11:24 - 11:26
    contemplated both for their elegant shape
  • 11:26 - 11:29
    and their virtuoso craftsmanship.
  • 11:30 - 11:32
    Hand axes mark
  • 11:32 - 11:34
    an evolutionary advance in human history --
  • 11:34 - 11:36
    tools fashioned to function
  • 11:36 - 11:39
    as what Darwinians call "fitness signals" --
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    that is to say, displays
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    that are performances
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    like the peacock's tail,
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    except that, unlike hair and feathers,
  • 11:48 - 11:50
    the hand axes are consciously
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    cleverly crafted.
  • 11:52 - 11:54
    Competently made hand axes
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    indicated desirable personal qualities --
  • 11:58 - 12:01
    intelligence, fine motor control,
  • 12:01 - 12:03
    planning ability,
  • 12:03 - 12:05
    conscientiousness
  • 12:05 - 12:08
    and sometimes access to rare materials.
  • 12:08 - 12:11
    Over tens of thousands of generations,
  • 12:11 - 12:13
    such skills increased the status
  • 12:13 - 12:15
    of those who displayed them
  • 12:15 - 12:17
    and gained a reproductive advantage
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    over the less capable.
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    You know, it's an old line,
  • 12:21 - 12:23
    but it has been shown to work --
  • 12:23 - 12:26
    "Why don't you come up to my cave, so I can show you my hand axes?"
  • 12:26 - 12:28
    (Laughter)
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    Except, of course, what's interesting about this
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    is that we can't be sure how that idea was conveyed,
  • 12:34 - 12:36
    because the Homo erectus
  • 12:36 - 12:39
    that made these objects
  • 12:39 - 12:41
    did not have language.
  • 12:41 - 12:43
    It's hard to grasp,
  • 12:43 - 12:46
    but it's an incredible fact.
  • 12:46 - 12:48
    This object was made
  • 12:48 - 12:51
    by a hominid ancestor,
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    Homo erectus or Homo ergaster,
  • 12:55 - 12:58
    between 50,000 and 100,000 years
  • 12:58 - 13:00
    before language.
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    Stretching over a million years,
  • 13:03 - 13:05
    the hand axe tradition
  • 13:05 - 13:08
    is the longest artistic tradition
  • 13:08 - 13:11
    in human and proto-human history.
  • 13:11 - 13:14
    By the end of the hand axe epic, Homo sapiens --
  • 13:14 - 13:16
    as they were then called, finally --
  • 13:16 - 13:18
    were doubtless finding new ways
  • 13:18 - 13:21
    to amuse and amaze each other
  • 13:21 - 13:23
    by, who knows, telling jokes,
  • 13:23 - 13:26
    storytelling, dancing, or hairstyling.
  • 13:26 - 13:29
    Yes, hairstyling -- I insist on that.
  • 13:29 - 13:31
    For us moderns,
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    virtuoso technique
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    is used to create imaginary worlds
  • 13:35 - 13:37
    in fiction and in movies,
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    to express intense emotions
  • 13:39 - 13:42
    with music, painting and dance.
  • 13:42 - 13:44
    But still,
  • 13:44 - 13:46
    one fundamental trait
  • 13:46 - 13:48
    of the ancestral personality persists
  • 13:48 - 13:51
    in our aesthetic cravings:
  • 13:51 - 13:53
    the beauty we find
  • 13:53 - 13:55
    in skilled performances.
  • 13:55 - 13:57
    From Lascaux to the Louvre
  • 13:57 - 13:59
    to Carnegie Hall,
  • 13:59 - 14:01
    human beings
  • 14:01 - 14:03
    have a permanent innate taste
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    for virtuoso displays in the arts.
  • 14:07 - 14:09
    We find beauty
  • 14:09 - 14:11
    in something done well.
  • 14:13 - 14:15
    So the next time you pass a jewelry shop window
  • 14:15 - 14:17
    displaying a beautifully cut
  • 14:17 - 14:19
    teardrop-shaped stone,
  • 14:19 - 14:21
    don't be so sure
  • 14:21 - 14:23
    it's just your culture telling you
  • 14:23 - 14:25
    that that sparkling jewel is beautiful.
  • 14:25 - 14:28
    Your distant ancestors loved that shape
  • 14:28 - 14:31
    and found beauty in the skill needed to make it,
  • 14:31 - 14:33
    even before
  • 14:33 - 14:35
    they could put their love into words.
  • 14:35 - 14:38
    Is beauty in the eye of the beholder?
  • 14:38 - 14:41
    No, it's deep in our minds.
  • 14:41 - 14:44
    It's a gift handed down from the intelligent skills
  • 14:44 - 14:46
    and rich emotional lives
  • 14:46 - 14:49
    of our most ancient ancestors.
  • 14:49 - 14:51
    Our powerful reaction to images,
  • 14:51 - 14:54
    to the expression of emotion in art,
  • 14:54 - 14:57
    to the beauty of music, to the night sky,
  • 14:57 - 15:00
    will be with us and our descendants
  • 15:00 - 15:03
    for as long as the human race exists.
  • 15:03 - 15:05
    Thank you.
  • 15:05 - 15:12
    (Applause)
Title:
A Darwinian theory of beauty
Speaker:
Denis Dutton
Description:

TED collaborates with animator Andrew Park to illustrate Denis Dutton's provocative theory on beauty -- that art, music and other beautiful things, far from being simply "in the eye of the beholder," are a core part of human nature with deep evolutionary origins.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
15:13
TED edited English subtitles for A Darwinian theory of beauty
TED added a translation

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions