3 clues to understanding your brain
-
0:00 - 0:04Well, as Chris pointed out, I study the human brain,
-
0:04 - 0:06the functions and structure of the human brain.
-
0:06 - 0:10And I just want you to think for a minute about what this entails.
-
0:10 - 0:14Here is this mass of jelly, three-pound mass of jelly
-
0:14 - 0:17you can hold in the palm of your hand,
-
0:17 - 0:21and it can contemplate the vastness of interstellar space.
-
0:21 - 0:23It can contemplate the meaning of infinity
-
0:23 - 0:28and it can contemplate itself contemplating on the meaning of infinity.
-
0:28 - 0:33And this peculiar recursive quality that we call self-awareness,
-
0:33 - 0:37which I think is the holy grail of neuroscience, of neurology,
-
0:37 - 0:39and hopefully, someday, we'll understand how that happens.
-
0:40 - 0:43OK, so how do you study this mysterious organ?
-
0:43 - 0:47I mean, you have 100 billion nerve cells,
-
0:47 - 0:50little wisps of protoplasm, interacting with each other,
-
0:50 - 0:54and from this activity emerges the whole spectrum of abilities
-
0:54 - 0:57that we call human nature and human consciousness.
-
0:57 - 0:58How does this happen?
-
0:58 - 1:01Well, there are many ways of approaching the functions of the human brain.
-
1:01 - 1:04One approach, the one we use mainly,
-
1:04 - 1:09is to look at patients with sustained damage to a small region of the brain,
-
1:09 - 1:11where there's been a genetic change in a small region of the brain.
-
1:11 - 1:15What then happens is not an across-the-board reduction
-
1:15 - 1:17in all your mental capacities,
-
1:17 - 1:20a sort of blunting of your cognitive ability.
-
1:20 - 1:23What you get is a highly selective loss of one function,
-
1:23 - 1:25with other functions being preserved intact,
-
1:25 - 1:27and this gives you some confidence in asserting
-
1:27 - 1:31that that part of the brain is somehow involved in mediating that function.
-
1:31 - 1:33So you can then map function onto structure,
-
1:33 - 1:36and then find out what the circuitry's doing
-
1:36 - 1:38to generate that particular function.
-
1:38 - 1:40So that's what we're trying to do.
-
1:40 - 1:43So let me give you a few striking examples of this.
-
1:43 - 1:47In fact, I'm giving you three examples, six minutes each, during this talk.
-
1:47 - 1:51The first example is an extraordinary syndrome called Capgras syndrome.
-
1:51 - 1:53If you look at the first slide there,
-
1:53 - 1:58that's the temporal lobes, frontal lobes, parietal lobes, OK --
-
1:58 - 2:00the lobes that constitute the brain.
-
2:00 - 2:04And if you look, tucked away inside the inner surface of the temporal lobes --
-
2:04 - 2:06you can't see it there --
-
2:06 - 2:08is a little structure called the fusiform gyrus.
-
2:08 - 2:11And that's been called the face area in the brain,
-
2:11 - 2:14because when it's damaged, you can no longer recognize people's faces.
-
2:14 - 2:16You can still recognize them from their voice
-
2:16 - 2:18and say, "Oh yeah, that's Joe,"
-
2:18 - 2:21but you can't look at their face and know who it is, right?
-
2:21 - 2:23You can't even recognize yourself in the mirror.
-
2:23 - 2:26I mean, you know it's you because you wink and it winks,
-
2:26 - 2:28and you know it's a mirror,
-
2:28 - 2:31but you don't really recognize yourself as yourself.
-
2:31 - 2:35OK. Now that syndrome is well known as caused by damage to the fusiform gyrus.
-
2:35 - 2:38But there's another rare syndrome, so rare, in fact,
-
2:38 - 2:42that very few physicians have heard about it, not even neurologists.
-
2:42 - 2:44This is called the Capgras delusion,
-
2:44 - 2:47and that is a patient, who's otherwise completely normal,
-
2:47 - 2:50has had a head injury, comes out of coma,
-
2:50 - 2:53otherwise completely normal, he looks at his mother
-
2:53 - 2:56and says, "This looks exactly like my mother, this woman,
-
2:56 - 2:58but she's an impostor.
-
2:58 - 3:00She's some other woman pretending to be my mother."
-
3:00 - 3:02Now, why does this happen?
-
3:02 - 3:05Why would somebody -- and this person is perfectly lucid and intelligent
-
3:05 - 3:07in all other respects, but when he sees his mother,
-
3:07 - 3:10his delusion kicks in and says, it's not mother.
-
3:10 - 3:12Now, the most common interpretation of this,
-
3:12 - 3:14which you find in all the psychiatry textbooks,
-
3:14 - 3:18is a Freudian view, and that is that this chap --
-
3:18 - 3:20and the same argument applies to women, by the way,
-
3:20 - 3:22but I'll just talk about guys.
-
3:22 - 3:25When you're a little baby, a young baby,
-
3:25 - 3:27you had a strong sexual attraction to your mother.
-
3:27 - 3:29This is the so-called Oedipus complex of Freud.
-
3:29 - 3:31I'm not saying I believe this,
-
3:31 - 3:33but this is the standard Freudian view.
-
3:33 - 3:36And then, as you grow up, the cortex develops,
-
3:36 - 3:40and inhibits these latent sexual urges towards your mother.
-
3:40 - 3:44Thank God, or you would all be sexually aroused when you saw your mother.
-
3:44 - 3:46And then what happens is,
-
3:46 - 3:48there's a blow to your head, damaging the cortex,
-
3:48 - 3:52allowing these latent sexual urges to emerge,
-
3:52 - 3:55flaming to the surface, and suddenly and inexplicably
-
3:55 - 3:58you find yourself being sexually aroused by your mother.
-
3:58 - 4:00And you say, "My God, if this is my mom,
-
4:00 - 4:02how come I'm being sexually turned on?
-
4:02 - 4:04She's some other woman. She's an impostor."
-
4:04 - 4:08It's the only interpretation that makes sense to your damaged brain.
-
4:08 - 4:11This has never made much sense to me, this argument.
-
4:11 - 4:14It's very ingenious, as all Freudian arguments are --
-
4:14 - 4:16(Laughter)
-
4:16 - 4:21-- but didn't make much sense because I have seen the same delusion,
-
4:21 - 4:23a patient having the same delusion, about his pet poodle.
-
4:23 - 4:24(Laughter)
-
4:24 - 4:29He'll say, "Doctor, this is not Fifi. It looks exactly like Fifi,
-
4:29 - 4:31but it's some other dog." Right?
-
4:31 - 4:33Now, you try using the Freudian explanation there.
-
4:33 - 4:34(Laughter)
-
4:34 - 4:38You'll start talking about the latent bestiality in all humans,
-
4:38 - 4:41or some such thing, which is quite absurd, of course.
-
4:41 - 4:43Now, what's really going on?
-
4:43 - 4:45So, to explain this curious disorder,
-
4:45 - 4:49we look at the structure and functions of the normal visual pathways in the brain.
-
4:49 - 4:52Normally, visual signals come in, into the eyeballs,
-
4:52 - 4:54go to the visual areas in the brain.
-
4:54 - 4:57There are, in fact, 30 areas in the back of your brain concerned with just vision,
-
4:57 - 5:00and after processing all that, the message goes to a small structure
-
5:00 - 5:05called the fusiform gyrus, where you perceive faces.
-
5:05 - 5:07There are neurons there that are sensitive to faces.
-
5:07 - 5:10You can call it the face area of the brain, right?
-
5:10 - 5:12I talked about that earlier.
-
5:12 - 5:16Now, when that area's damaged, you lose the ability to see faces, right?
-
5:16 - 5:19But from that area, the message cascades
-
5:19 - 5:22into a structure called the amygdala in the limbic system,
-
5:22 - 5:24the emotional core of the brain,
-
5:24 - 5:26and that structure, called the amygdala,
-
5:26 - 5:28gauges the emotional significance of what you're looking at.
-
5:28 - 5:32Is it prey? Is it predator? Is it mate?
-
5:32 - 5:34Or is it something absolutely trivial, like a piece of lint,
-
5:34 - 5:38or a piece of chalk, or a -- I don't want to point to that, but --
-
5:38 - 5:40or a shoe, or something like that? OK?
-
5:40 - 5:42Which you can completely ignore.
-
5:42 - 5:45So if the amygdala is excited, and this is something important,
-
5:45 - 5:48the messages then cascade into the autonomic nervous system.
-
5:48 - 5:50Your heart starts beating faster.
-
5:50 - 5:53You start sweating to dissipate the heat that you're going to
-
5:53 - 5:55create from muscular exertion.
-
5:55 - 5:59And that's fortunate, because we can put two electrodes on your palm
-
5:59 - 6:03and measure the change in skin resistance produced by sweating.
-
6:03 - 6:05So I can determine, when you're looking at something,
-
6:05 - 6:09whether you're excited or whether you're aroused, or not, OK?
-
6:09 - 6:11And I'll get to that in a minute.
-
6:11 - 6:15So my idea was, when this chap looks at an object, when he looks at his --
-
6:15 - 6:19any object for that matter, it goes to the visual areas and,
-
6:19 - 6:22however, and it's processed in the fusiform gyrus,
-
6:22 - 6:25and you recognize it as a pea plant, or a table,
-
6:25 - 6:27or your mother, for that matter, OK?
-
6:27 - 6:30And then the message cascades into the amygdala,
-
6:30 - 6:32and then goes down the autonomic nervous system.
-
6:32 - 6:37But maybe, in this chap, that wire that goes from the amygdala to the limbic system,
-
6:37 - 6:40the emotional core of the brain, is cut by the accident.
-
6:40 - 6:42So because the fusiform is intact,
-
6:42 - 6:45the chap can still recognize his mother,
-
6:45 - 6:47and says, "Oh yeah, this looks like my mother."
-
6:47 - 6:50But because the wire is cut to the emotional centers,
-
6:50 - 6:54he says, "But how come, if it's my mother, I don't experience a warmth?"
-
6:54 - 6:56Or terror, as the case may be? Right?
-
6:56 - 6:57(Laughter)
-
6:57 - 7:03And therefore, he says, "How do I account for this inexplicable lack of emotions?
-
7:03 - 7:05This can't be my mother.
-
7:05 - 7:07It's some strange woman pretending to be my mother."
-
7:07 - 7:09How do you test this?
-
7:09 - 7:11Well, what you do is, if you take any one of you here, and put you in front of a screen,
-
7:11 - 7:14and measure your galvanic skin response,
-
7:14 - 7:16and show pictures on the screen,
-
7:16 - 7:19I can measure how you sweat when you see an object,
-
7:19 - 7:22like a table or an umbrella. Of course, you don't sweat.
-
7:22 - 7:27If I show you a picture of a lion, or a tiger, or a pinup, you start sweating, right?
-
7:27 - 7:30And, believe it or not, if I show you a picture of your mother --
-
7:30 - 7:32I'm talking about normal people -- you start sweating.
-
7:32 - 7:34You don't even have to be Jewish.
-
7:34 - 7:36(Laughter)
-
7:36 - 7:40Now, what happens if you show this patient?
-
7:40 - 7:44You take the patient and show him pictures on the screen
-
7:44 - 7:46and measure his galvanic skin response.
-
7:46 - 7:51Tables and chairs and lint, nothing happens, as in normal people,
-
7:51 - 7:53but when you show him a picture of his mother,
-
7:53 - 7:55the galvanic skin response is flat.
-
7:55 - 7:57There's no emotional reaction to his mother,
-
7:57 - 8:02because that wire going from the visual areas to the emotional centers is cut.
-
8:02 - 8:05So his vision is normal because the visual areas are normal,
-
8:05 - 8:08his emotions are normal -- he'll laugh, he'll cry, so on and so forth --
-
8:08 - 8:11but the wire from vision to emotions is cut
-
8:11 - 8:14and therefore he has this delusion that his mother is an impostor.
-
8:14 - 8:17It's a lovely example of the sort of thing we do:
-
8:17 - 8:21take a bizarre, seemingly incomprehensible, neural psychiatric syndrome
-
8:21 - 8:23and say that the standard Freudian view is wrong,
-
8:23 - 8:27that, in fact, you can come up with a precise explanation
-
8:27 - 8:29in terms of the known neural anatomy of the brain.
-
8:29 - 8:31By the way, if this patient then goes,
-
8:31 - 8:36and mother phones from an adjacent room -- phones him --
-
8:36 - 8:40and he picks up the phone, and he says, "Wow, mom, how are you? Where are you?"
-
8:40 - 8:42There's no delusion through the phone.
-
8:42 - 8:44Then, she approaches him after an hour, he says, "Who are you?
-
8:44 - 8:46You look just like my mother." OK?
-
8:46 - 8:48The reason is there's a separate pathway
-
8:48 - 8:52going from the hearing centers in the brain to the emotional centers,
-
8:52 - 8:54and that's not been cut by the accident.
-
8:54 - 8:59So this explains why through the phone he recognizes his mother, no problem.
-
8:59 - 9:02When he sees her in person, he says it's an impostor.
-
9:02 - 9:06OK, how is all this complex circuitry set up in the brain?
-
9:06 - 9:09Is it nature, genes, or is it nurture?
-
9:09 - 9:11And we approach this problem
-
9:11 - 9:15by considering another curious syndrome called phantom limb.
-
9:15 - 9:17And you all know what a phantom limb is.
-
9:17 - 9:20When an arm is amputated, or a leg is amputated, for gangrene,
-
9:20 - 9:22or you lose it in war -- for example, in the Iraq war,
-
9:22 - 9:24it's now a serious problem --
-
9:24 - 9:28you continue to vividly feel the presence of that missing arm,
-
9:28 - 9:31and that's called a phantom arm or a phantom leg.
-
9:31 - 9:33In fact, you can get a phantom with almost any part of the body.
-
9:33 - 9:36Believe it or not, even with internal viscera.
-
9:36 - 9:40I've had patients with the uterus removed -- hysterectomy --
-
9:40 - 9:45who have a phantom uterus, including phantom menstrual cramps
-
9:45 - 9:47at the appropriate time of the month.
-
9:47 - 9:49And in fact, one student asked me the other day,
-
9:49 - 9:51"Do they get phantom PMS?"
-
9:51 - 9:52(Laughter)
-
9:52 - 9:56A subject ripe for scientific enquiry, but we haven't pursued that.
-
9:56 - 9:59OK, now the next question is,
-
9:59 - 10:02what can you learn about phantom limbs by doing experiments?
-
10:02 - 10:04One of the things we've found was,
-
10:04 - 10:06about half the patients with phantom limbs
-
10:06 - 10:08claim that they can move the phantom.
-
10:08 - 10:10It'll pat his brother on the shoulder,
-
10:10 - 10:12it'll answer the phone when it rings, it'll wave goodbye.
-
10:12 - 10:15These are very compelling, vivid sensations.
-
10:15 - 10:17The patient's not delusional.
-
10:17 - 10:19He knows that the arm is not there,
-
10:19 - 10:22but, nevertheless, it's a compelling sensory experience for the patient.
-
10:22 - 10:25But however, about half the patients, this doesn't happen.
-
10:25 - 10:29The phantom limb -- they'll say, "But doctor, the phantom limb is paralyzed.
-
10:29 - 10:32It's fixed in a clenched spasm and it's excruciatingly painful.
-
10:32 - 10:35If only I could move it, maybe the pain will be relieved."
-
10:35 - 10:38Now, why would a phantom limb be paralyzed?
-
10:38 - 10:40It sounds like an oxymoron.
-
10:40 - 10:43But when we were looking at the case sheets, what we found was,
-
10:43 - 10:45these people with the paralyzed phantom limbs,
-
10:45 - 10:49the original arm was paralyzed because of the peripheral nerve injury.
-
10:49 - 10:52The actual nerve supplying the arm was severed,
-
10:52 - 10:54was cut, by say, a motorcycle accident.
-
10:54 - 10:57So the patient had an actual arm, which is painful,
-
10:57 - 11:01in a sling for a few months or a year, and then,
-
11:01 - 11:04in a misguided attempt to get rid of the pain in the arm,
-
11:04 - 11:06the surgeon amputates the arm,
-
11:06 - 11:10and then you get a phantom arm with the same pains, right?
-
11:10 - 11:12And this is a serious clinical problem.
-
11:12 - 11:14Patients become depressed.
-
11:14 - 11:16Some of them are driven to suicide, OK?
-
11:16 - 11:18So, how do you treat this syndrome?
-
11:18 - 11:20Now, why do you get a paralyzed phantom limb?
-
11:20 - 11:24When I looked at the case sheet, I found that they had an actual arm,
-
11:24 - 11:27and the nerves supplying the arm had been cut,
-
11:27 - 11:30and the actual arm had been paralyzed,
-
11:30 - 11:34and lying in a sling for several months before the amputation,
-
11:34 - 11:40and this pain then gets carried over into the phantom itself.
-
11:40 - 11:42Why does this happen?
-
11:42 - 11:44When the arm was intact, but paralyzed,
-
11:44 - 11:47the brain sends commands to the arm, the front of the brain, saying, "Move,"
-
11:47 - 11:49but it's getting visual feedback saying, "No."
-
11:49 - 11:53Move. No. Move. No. Move. No.
-
11:53 - 11:56And this gets wired into the circuitry of the brain,
-
11:56 - 11:59and we call this learned paralysis, OK?
-
11:59 - 12:03The brain learns, because of this Hebbian, associative link,
-
12:03 - 12:06that the mere command to move the arm
-
12:06 - 12:08creates a sensation of a paralyzed arm.
-
12:08 - 12:10And then, when you've amputated the arm,
-
12:10 - 12:14this learned paralysis carries over into your body image
-
12:14 - 12:17and into your phantom, OK?
-
12:17 - 12:19Now, how do you help these patients?
-
12:19 - 12:21How do you unlearn the learned paralysis,
-
12:21 - 12:25so you can relieve him of this excruciating, clenching spasm
-
12:25 - 12:27of the phantom arm?
-
12:27 - 12:32Well, we said, what if you now send the command to the phantom,
-
12:32 - 12:36but give him visual feedback that it's obeying his command, right?
-
12:36 - 12:39Maybe you can relieve the phantom pain, the phantom cramp.
-
12:39 - 12:41How do you do that? Well, virtual reality.
-
12:41 - 12:43But that costs millions of dollars.
-
12:43 - 12:46So, I hit on a way of doing this for three dollars,
-
12:46 - 12:48but don't tell my funding agencies.
-
12:48 - 12:49(Laughter)
-
12:49 - 12:53OK? What you do is you create what I call a mirror box.
-
12:53 - 12:55You have a cardboard box with a mirror in the middle,
-
12:55 - 12:59and then you put the phantom -- so my first patient, Derek, came in.
-
12:59 - 13:02He had his arm amputated 10 years ago.
-
13:02 - 13:05He had a brachial avulsion, so the nerves were cut
-
13:05 - 13:09and the arm was paralyzed, lying in a sling for a year, and then the arm was amputated.
-
13:09 - 13:11He had a phantom arm, excruciatingly painful, and he couldn't move it.
-
13:11 - 13:13It was a paralyzed phantom arm.
-
13:13 - 13:17So he came there, and I gave him a mirror like that, in a box,
-
13:17 - 13:20which I call a mirror box, right?
-
13:20 - 13:23And the patient puts his phantom left arm,
-
13:23 - 13:25which is clenched and in spasm, on the left side of the mirror,
-
13:25 - 13:27and the normal hand on the right side of the mirror,
-
13:27 - 13:31and makes the same posture, the clenched posture,
-
13:31 - 13:34and looks inside the mirror. And what does he experience?
-
13:34 - 13:37He looks at the phantom being resurrected,
-
13:37 - 13:41because he's looking at the reflection of the normal arm in the mirror,
-
13:41 - 13:43and it looks like this phantom has been resurrected.
-
13:43 - 13:46"Now," I said, "now, look, wiggle your phantom --
-
13:46 - 13:50your real fingers, or move your real fingers while looking in the mirror."
-
13:50 - 13:54He's going to get the visual impression that the phantom is moving, right?
-
13:54 - 13:56That's obvious, but the astonishing thing is,
-
13:56 - 13:59the patient then says, "Oh my God, my phantom is moving again,
-
13:59 - 14:01and the pain, the clenching spasm, is relieved."
-
14:01 - 14:04And remember, my first patient who came in --
-
14:04 - 14:05(Applause)
-
14:05 - 14:09-- thank you. (Applause)
-
14:09 - 14:12My first patient came in, and he looked in the mirror,
-
14:12 - 14:15and I said, "Look at your reflection of your phantom."
-
14:15 - 14:17And he started giggling, he says, "I can see my phantom."
-
14:17 - 14:19But he's not stupid. He knows it's not real.
-
14:19 - 14:21He knows it's a mirror reflection,
-
14:21 - 14:23but it's a vivid sensory experience.
-
14:23 - 14:26Now, I said, "Move your normal hand and phantom."
-
14:26 - 14:28He said, "Oh, I can't move my phantom. You know that. It's painful."
-
14:28 - 14:30I said, "Move your normal hand."
-
14:30 - 14:32And he says, "Oh my God, my phantom is moving again. I don't believe this!
-
14:32 - 14:35And my pain is being relieved." OK?
-
14:35 - 14:36And then I said, "Close your eyes."
-
14:36 - 14:38He closes his eyes.
-
14:38 - 14:39"And move your normal hand."
-
14:39 - 14:40"Oh, nothing. It's clenched again."
-
14:40 - 14:42"OK, open your eyes."
-
14:42 - 14:43"Oh my God, oh my God, it's moving again!"
-
14:43 - 14:45So, he was like a kid in a candy store.
-
14:45 - 14:50So, I said, OK, this proves my theory about learned paralysis
-
14:50 - 14:52and the critical role of visual input,
-
14:52 - 14:54but I'm not going to get a Nobel Prize
-
14:54 - 14:56for getting somebody to move his phantom limb.
-
14:56 - 14:57(Laughter)
-
14:57 - 14:58(Applause)
-
14:58 - 15:01It's a completely useless ability, if you think about it.
-
15:01 - 15:02(Laughter)
-
15:02 - 15:06But then I started realizing, maybe other kinds of paralysis
-
15:06 - 15:11that you see in neurology, like stroke, focal dystonias --
-
15:11 - 15:13there may be a learned component to this,
-
15:13 - 15:16which you can overcome with the simple device of using a mirror.
-
15:16 - 15:18So, I said, "Look, Derek" --
-
15:18 - 15:21well, first of all, the guy can't just go around carrying a mirror to alleviate his pain --
-
15:21 - 15:25I said, "Look, Derek, take it home and practice with it for a week or two.
-
15:25 - 15:27Maybe, after a period of practice,
-
15:27 - 15:29you can dispense with the mirror, unlearn the paralysis,
-
15:29 - 15:31and start moving your paralyzed arm,
-
15:31 - 15:33and then, relieve yourself of pain."
-
15:33 - 15:35So he said OK, and he took it home.
-
15:35 - 15:37I said, "Look, it's, after all, two dollars. Take it home."
-
15:37 - 15:40So, he took it home, and after two weeks, he phones me,
-
15:40 - 15:42and he said, "Doctor, you're not going to believe this."
-
15:42 - 15:43I said, "What?"
-
15:43 - 15:45He said, "It's gone."
-
15:45 - 15:46I said, "What's gone?"
-
15:46 - 15:48I thought maybe the mirror box was gone.
-
15:48 - 15:49(Laughter)
-
15:49 - 15:52He said, "No, no, no, you know this phantom I've had for the last 10 years?
-
15:52 - 15:54It's disappeared."
-
15:54 - 15:56And I said -- I got worried, I said, my God,
-
15:56 - 15:58I mean I've changed this guy's body image,
-
15:58 - 16:01what about human subjects, ethics and all of that?
-
16:01 - 16:03And I said, "Derek, does this bother you?"
-
16:03 - 16:06He said, "No, last three days, I've not had a phantom arm
-
16:06 - 16:09and therefore no phantom elbow pain, no clenching,
-
16:09 - 16:12no phantom forearm pain, all those pains are gone away.
-
16:12 - 16:16But the problem is I still have my phantom fingers dangling from the shoulder,
-
16:16 - 16:18and your box doesn't reach."
-
16:18 - 16:19(Laughter)
-
16:19 - 16:22"So, can you change the design and put it on my forehead,
-
16:22 - 16:25so I can, you know, do this and eliminate my phantom fingers?"
-
16:25 - 16:27He thought I was some kind of magician.
-
16:27 - 16:28Now, why does this happen?
-
16:28 - 16:31It's because the brain is faced with tremendous sensory conflict.
-
16:31 - 16:34It's getting messages from vision saying the phantom is back.
-
16:34 - 16:36On the other hand, there's no proprioception,
-
16:36 - 16:40muscle signals saying that there is no arm, right?
-
16:40 - 16:42And your motor command saying there is an arm,
-
16:42 - 16:45and, because of this conflict, the brain says, to hell with it,
-
16:45 - 16:48there is no phantom, there is no arm, right?
-
16:48 - 16:50It goes into a sort of denial -- it gates the signals.
-
16:50 - 16:54And when the arm disappears, the bonus is, the pain disappears
-
16:54 - 16:58because you can't have disembodied pain floating out there, in space.
-
16:58 - 17:00So, that's the bonus.
-
17:00 - 17:02Now, this technique has been tried on dozens of patients
-
17:02 - 17:04by other groups in Helsinki,
-
17:04 - 17:07so it may prove to be valuable as a treatment for phantom pain,
-
17:07 - 17:09and indeed, people have tried it for stroke rehabilitation.
-
17:09 - 17:12Stroke you normally think of as damage to the fibers,
-
17:12 - 17:14nothing you can do about it.
-
17:14 - 17:19But, it turns out some component of stroke paralysis is also learned paralysis,
-
17:19 - 17:22and maybe that component can be overcome using mirrors.
-
17:22 - 17:24This has also gone through clinical trials,
-
17:24 - 17:26helping lots and lots of patients.
-
17:26 - 17:30OK, let me switch gears now to the third part of my talk,
-
17:30 - 17:34which is about another curious phenomenon called synesthesia.
-
17:34 - 17:37This was discovered by Francis Galton in the nineteenth century.
-
17:37 - 17:39He was a cousin of Charles Darwin.
-
17:39 - 17:41He pointed out that certain people in the population,
-
17:41 - 17:45who are otherwise completely normal, had the following peculiarity:
-
17:45 - 17:48every time they see a number, it's colored.
-
17:48 - 17:52Five is blue, seven is yellow, eight is chartreuse,
-
17:52 - 17:54nine is indigo, OK?
-
17:54 - 17:57Bear in mind, these people are completely normal in other respects.
-
17:57 - 18:00Or C sharp -- sometimes, tones evoke color.
-
18:00 - 18:03C sharp is blue, F sharp is green,
-
18:03 - 18:06another tone might be yellow, right?
-
18:06 - 18:08Why does this happen?
-
18:08 - 18:10This is called synesthesia. Galton called it synesthesia,
-
18:10 - 18:12a mingling of the senses.
-
18:12 - 18:14In us, all the senses are distinct.
-
18:14 - 18:16These people muddle up their senses.
-
18:16 - 18:17Why does this happen?
-
18:17 - 18:19One of the two aspects of this problem are very intriguing.
-
18:19 - 18:21Synesthesia runs in families,
-
18:21 - 18:24so Galton said this is a hereditary basis, a genetic basis.
-
18:24 - 18:28Secondly, synesthesia is about -- and this is what gets me to my point
-
18:28 - 18:31about the main theme of this lecture, which is about creativity --
-
18:31 - 18:36synesthesia is eight times more common among artists, poets, novelists
-
18:36 - 18:39and other creative people than in the general population.
-
18:39 - 18:40Why would that be?
-
18:40 - 18:42I'm going to answer that question.
-
18:42 - 18:44It's never been answered before.
-
18:44 - 18:45OK, what is synesthesia? What causes it?
-
18:45 - 18:46Well, there are many theories.
-
18:46 - 18:48One theory is they're just crazy.
-
18:48 - 18:51Now, that's not really a scientific theory, so we can forget about it.
-
18:51 - 18:55Another theory is they are acid junkies and potheads, right?
-
18:55 - 18:57Now, there may be some truth to this,
-
18:57 - 18:59because it's much more common here in the Bay Area than in San Diego.
-
18:59 - 19:00(Laughter)
-
19:00 - 19:03OK. Now, the third theory is that --
-
19:03 - 19:08well, let's ask ourselves what's really going on in synesthesia. All right?
-
19:08 - 19:11So, we found that the color area and the number area
-
19:11 - 19:14are right next to each other in the brain, in the fusiform gyrus.
-
19:14 - 19:16So we said, there's some accidental cross wiring
-
19:16 - 19:19between color and numbers in the brain.
-
19:19 - 19:22So, every time you see a number, you see a corresponding color,
-
19:22 - 19:24and that's why you get synesthesia.
-
19:24 - 19:26Now remember -- why does this happen?
-
19:26 - 19:28Why would there be crossed wires in some people?
-
19:28 - 19:30Remember I said it runs in families?
-
19:30 - 19:32That gives you the clue.
-
19:32 - 19:34And that is, there is an abnormal gene,
-
19:34 - 19:37a mutation in the gene that causes this abnormal cross wiring.
-
19:37 - 19:39In all of us, it turns out
-
19:39 - 19:43we are born with everything wired to everything else.
-
19:43 - 19:46So, every brain region is wired to every other region,
-
19:46 - 19:48and these are trimmed down to create
-
19:48 - 19:51the characteristic modular architecture of the adult brain.
-
19:51 - 19:53So, if there's a gene causing this trimming
-
19:53 - 19:55and if that gene mutates,
-
19:55 - 19:58then you get deficient trimming between adjacent brain areas.
-
19:58 - 20:01And if it's between number and color, you get number-color synesthesia.
-
20:01 - 20:04If it's between tone and color, you get tone-color synesthesia.
-
20:04 - 20:06So far, so good.
-
20:06 - 20:08Now, what if this gene is expressed everywhere in the brain,
-
20:08 - 20:09so everything is cross-connected?
-
20:09 - 20:15Well, think about what artists, novelists and poets have in common,
-
20:15 - 20:18the ability to engage in metaphorical thinking,
-
20:18 - 20:20linking seemingly unrelated ideas,
-
20:20 - 20:23such as, "It is the east, and Juliet is the Sun."
-
20:23 - 20:25Well, you don't say, Juliet is the sun,
-
20:25 - 20:27does that mean she's a glowing ball of fire?
-
20:27 - 20:30I mean, schizophrenics do that, but it's a different story, right?
-
20:30 - 20:33Normal people say, she's warm like the sun,
-
20:33 - 20:35she's radiant like the sun, she's nurturing like the sun.
-
20:35 - 20:37Instantly, you've found the links.
-
20:37 - 20:40Now, if you assume that this greater cross wiring
-
20:40 - 20:43and concepts are also in different parts of the brain,
-
20:43 - 20:46then it's going to create a greater propensity
-
20:46 - 20:49towards metaphorical thinking and creativity
-
20:49 - 20:51in people with synesthesia.
-
20:51 - 20:54And, hence, the eight times more common incidence of synesthesia
-
20:54 - 20:56among poets, artists and novelists.
-
20:56 - 20:59OK, it's a very phrenological view of synesthesia.
-
20:59 - 21:01The last demonstration -- can I take one minute?
-
21:01 - 21:03(Applause)
-
21:03 - 21:08OK. I'm going to show you that you're all synesthetes, but you're in denial about it.
-
21:08 - 21:12Here's what I call Martian alphabet. Just like your alphabet,
-
21:12 - 21:15A is A, B is B, C is C.
-
21:15 - 21:18Different shapes for different phonemes, right?
-
21:18 - 21:20Here, you've got Martian alphabet.
-
21:20 - 21:22One of them is Kiki, one of them is Bouba.
-
21:22 - 21:24Which one is Kiki and which one is Bouba?
-
21:24 - 21:26How many of you think that's Kiki and that's Bouba? Raise your hands.
-
21:26 - 21:28Well, it's one or two mutants.
-
21:28 - 21:29(Laughter)
-
21:29 - 21:31How many of you think that's Bouba, that's Kiki? Raise your hands.
-
21:31 - 21:33Ninety-nine percent of you.
-
21:33 - 21:35Now, none of you is a Martian. How did you do that?
-
21:35 - 21:40It's because you're all doing a cross-model synesthetic abstraction,
-
21:40 - 21:44meaning you're saying that that sharp inflection -- ki-ki,
-
21:44 - 21:49in your auditory cortex, the hair cells being excited -- Kiki,
-
21:49 - 21:52mimics the visual inflection, sudden inflection of that jagged shape.
-
21:52 - 21:55Now, this is very important, because what it's telling you
-
21:55 - 21:57is your brain is engaging in a primitive --
-
21:57 - 21:59it's just -- it looks like a silly illusion,
-
21:59 - 22:03but these photons in your eye are doing this shape,
-
22:03 - 22:06and hair cells in your ear are exciting the auditory pattern,
-
22:06 - 22:11but the brain is able to extract the common denominator.
-
22:11 - 22:13It's a primitive form of abstraction,
-
22:13 - 22:18and we now know this happens in the fusiform gyrus of the brain,
-
22:18 - 22:19because when that's damaged,
-
22:19 - 22:23these people lose the ability to engage in Bouba Kiki,
-
22:23 - 22:25but they also lose the ability to engage in metaphor.
-
22:25 - 22:29If you ask this guy, what -- "all that glitters is not gold,"
-
22:29 - 22:31what does that mean?"
-
22:31 - 22:33The patient says, "Well, if it's metallic and shiny, it doesn't mean it's gold.
-
22:33 - 22:36You have to measure its specific gravity, OK?"
-
22:36 - 22:39So, they completely miss the metaphorical meaning.
-
22:39 - 22:42So, this area is about eight times the size in higher --
-
22:42 - 22:45especially in humans -- as in lower primates.
-
22:45 - 22:48Something very interesting is going on here in the angular gyrus,
-
22:48 - 22:51because it's the crossroads between hearing, vision and touch,
-
22:51 - 22:55and it became enormous in humans. And something very interesting is going on.
-
22:55 - 22:58And I think it's a basis of many uniquely human abilities
-
22:58 - 23:01like abstraction, metaphor and creativity.
-
23:01 - 23:04All of these questions that philosophers have been studying for millennia,
-
23:04 - 23:08we scientists can begin to explore by doing brain imaging,
-
23:08 - 23:10and by studying patients and asking the right questions.
-
23:10 - 23:12Thank you.
-
23:12 - 23:13(Applause)
-
23:13 - 23:14Sorry about that.
-
23:14 - 23:15(Laughter)
- Title:
- 3 clues to understanding your brain
- Speaker:
- VS Ramachandran
- Description:
-
Vilayanur Ramachandran tells us what brain damage can reveal about the connection between celebral tissue and the mind, using three startling delusions as examples.
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
closed TED
- Project:
- TEDTalks
- Duration:
- 23:17
![]() |
Brian Greene edited English subtitles for 3 clues to understanding your brain | |
![]() |
Morton Bast edited English subtitles for 3 clues to understanding your brain | |
![]() |
Jenny Zurawell edited English subtitles for 3 clues to understanding your brain | |
![]() |
Jenny Zurawell edited English subtitles for 3 clues to understanding your brain | |
![]() |
TED added a translation |