Return to Video

Complexity | Eric Lumer | TEDxLakeComo

  • 0:07 - 0:11
    Tonight I want to explore the relationship
    between two complex phenomena.
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    Complexity is often a dynamic feature,
    rather than a static one.
  • 0:15 - 0:16
    It's a process.
  • 0:16 - 0:19
    As such, we heard it earlier on,
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    it’s very difficult to represent visually,
  • 0:22 - 0:26
    because it develops over time,
  • 0:26 - 0:31
    in many spatial dimensions or networks.
  • 0:31 - 0:32
    These two complex processes
  • 0:32 - 0:38
    are related to the systems and networks
    allowing communication on the Internet,
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    and to the neural bases of cognition.
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    In other words, how our brain works.
  • 0:48 - 0:52
    I was inspired to find a relationship
    between these two topics,
  • 0:52 - 0:56
    which I devoted most
    of my professional life to,
  • 0:56 - 1:01
    by a talk by Kevin Kelly,
    executive editor of Wired magazine,
  • 1:01 - 1:03
    given at TEDGlobal two years ago.
  • 1:04 - 1:08
    Kelly was discussing
    the first 5,000 days of the Web,
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    wondering about one question:
  • 1:12 - 1:16
    what can we expect
    in the next 5,000 days?
  • 1:20 - 1:25
    He highlighted the fact
    that in a very short time, a few years,
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    the Web has become
    an extremely complex system.
  • 1:29 - 1:30
    First, in terms of its structure.
  • 1:31 - 1:36
    For example, it contains
    55 trillion hypertext links between pages,
  • 1:36 - 1:42
    a number so big,
    it means almost nothing to us.
  • 1:43 - 1:48
    Second, in terms of the data
    that it’s continuously conveying:
  • 1:48 - 1:51
    100 billion clicks per day,
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    even before the boom of social networks
  • 1:54 - 1:58
    and the amount of information
    they are constantly exchanging.
  • 2:00 - 2:05
    So much so that these figures
  • 2:05 - 2:09
    are beginning to be comparable
    to those of a human brain.
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    If we look at the number of components
  • 2:12 - 2:16
    in this huge global machine
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    and compare it
  • 2:18 - 2:22
    with the neurons in the human brain,
  • 2:22 - 2:28
    we realize that the number
    of connections in the Web
  • 2:28 - 2:31
    is approaching the same order of magnitude
  • 2:31 - 2:35
    as that of synapses in our brains,
    at least as far as neural circuits go.
  • 2:37 - 2:43
    This brought Kelly
    to speculate on the fact that
  • 2:43 - 2:46
    on the one hand, this system is becoming
  • 2:46 - 2:50
    a way of constantly accessing information
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    for all human beings,
  • 2:53 - 2:54
    but on the other hand,
  • 2:54 - 2:58
    we're turning into collectors
    of information,
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    endless sensors of this giant machine.
  • 3:01 - 3:07
    We'll be going around with
    these "sixth sense" devices you've seen,
  • 3:07 - 3:12
    inputting information
    and sending messages over the Web.
  • 3:12 - 3:16
    In essence, telling it
    what’s happening around the world.
  • 3:16 - 3:22
    There'll be hundreds of millions of people
    doing it, soon maybe even billions.
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    The same applies to artificial sensors,
  • 3:26 - 3:32
    which continuously provide the Web
    with data on environmental conditions.
  • 3:32 - 3:33
    At the same time,
  • 3:33 - 3:37
    these same people and devices
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    will become actuators of the Web,
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    allowing it
  • 3:41 - 3:45
    to interact with the world
    and actively change its course.
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    Indeed, Kelly speculated on the fact
  • 3:48 - 3:52
    that we are actually
    developing a big machine,
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    what he calls the ONE, an ethereal entity,
  • 3:54 - 3:59
    which might start to display
    some features of human-like consciousness.
  • 4:01 - 4:06
    Having studied both topics,
    I got interested in this question
  • 4:06 - 4:07
    and I’m afraid I must disappoint you.
  • 4:08 - 4:12
    The Internet is a packed house
    but no one lives in it.
  • 4:12 - 4:17
    There’s nobody in there
    looking out at what’s happening.
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    It’s as conscious as my shoes.
  • 4:20 - 4:25
    However, what I’d like
    to illustrate tonight
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    is the fact that, gradually,
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    it's acquiring a series
    of traits and capabilities
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    that enable it to host
  • 4:33 - 4:38
    ever more coordinate collective processes
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    which have some of the features
    of human consciousness:
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    the ability to anticipate
    what's happening,
  • 4:43 - 4:49
    the ability to reason and take decisions,
  • 4:49 - 4:51
    even the ability to think about itself,
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    to be self-referential.
  • 4:53 - 4:55
    These collective processes,
  • 4:56 - 5:00
    as there will probably be more than one,
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    will have a deep impact
  • 5:03 - 5:07
    not only on the way our societies work,
  • 5:07 - 5:13
    but also on our very notion
    of social consciousness,
  • 5:13 - 5:17
    which is actually being redefined.
  • 5:19 - 5:22
    Let’s have a look
    at what makes up our brain
  • 5:22 - 5:28
    and at some of the ingredients which,
    while not sufficient per se,
  • 5:28 - 5:32
    are essential for the development
    of consciousness.
  • 5:32 - 5:35
    First of all, the ability to learn:
  • 5:35 - 5:39
    that is, the ability to interact
    with the external world,
  • 5:39 - 5:44
    and to absorb features, correlations,
  • 5:44 - 5:47
    and representations of the external world,
  • 5:47 - 5:52
    as well as the way we interact with it.
  • 5:52 - 5:57
    The most concrete
    manifestation of this ability
  • 5:57 - 6:01
    is the selection and modulation
    of connections among neurons.
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    These synapses are responsible
    for the propagation
  • 6:03 - 6:08
    of electrochemical pulses
    from one neuron to another
  • 6:08 - 6:12
    and for the creation
    of these systems in our brain
  • 6:12 - 6:16
    according to our senses
    and our locomotor systems.
  • 6:17 - 6:18
    Anyway, this plasticity,
  • 6:18 - 6:23
    this ability to modulate connections
    between neurons in the brain,
  • 6:23 - 6:28
    is not a passive system
    left to its own devices,
  • 6:28 - 6:34
    limited to absorbing relationships
    and correlations from the external world.
  • 6:34 - 6:38
    It’s a learning system
    which is driven by values,
  • 6:38 - 6:42
    by systems that are
    actually based on values.
  • 6:42 - 6:46
    Such value systems are located
    in the inner kernels of our brain.
  • 6:46 - 6:50
    These neural circuits lie
    in the archaic regions of the brain,
  • 6:50 - 6:54
    the same that we find in all mammals,
  • 6:54 - 6:58
    and deeply influence
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    the whole of the cerebral cortex.
  • 7:02 - 7:07
    They have a tendency to use data
    to create a raw interpretation of reality,
  • 7:07 - 7:12
    contrarily to what the cortex does.
  • 7:12 - 7:16
    It’s a basic interpretation
    of what’s happening around us,
  • 7:16 - 7:20
    an instinctive first impression
  • 7:20 - 7:23
    that attributes a value
    to what’s going on.
  • 7:23 - 7:28
    The cerebral cortex
    tends to build representations
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    integrating them with our conscience.
  • 7:31 - 7:35
    Instead, these neural circuits
    assign them a value.
  • 7:35 - 7:37
    For instance,
  • 7:37 - 7:44
    their activation can prompt us
    to satisfy our basic needs
  • 7:44 - 7:49
    such as eating, sleeping or reproducing.
  • 7:49 - 7:52
    The way these circuits work
  • 7:52 - 7:55
    is genetically inherited.
  • 7:55 - 8:00
    In turn, the cortex is capable
    of influencing such circuits
  • 8:00 - 8:01
    and modifying them
  • 8:01 - 8:05
    to create systems
    of so-called acquired values,
  • 8:05 - 8:06
    which aren't inherited
  • 8:06 - 8:10
    and are the cause of certain behaviours
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    that were also discussed here tonight.
  • 8:13 - 8:17
    For instance, the development of ethics,
  • 8:17 - 8:18
    shared concepts
  • 8:18 - 8:24
    which are largey acquired
    through social interaction.
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    The third element
  • 8:28 - 8:34
    is one of the key neural bases
    of consciousness.
  • 8:34 - 8:36
    It's the great ability to integrate
  • 8:36 - 8:41
    all modules and activity
    components within the brain.
  • 8:41 - 8:42
    Why is this?
  • 8:42 - 8:46
    Because consciousness is unitary.
  • 8:46 - 8:51
    It has to be, as it is strongly related
    to our ability to plan
  • 8:51 - 8:55
    and organize our interactions
    with the external world.
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    In addition, we have only one body.
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    Imagine what would happen
  • 9:01 - 9:05
    if there were two conscious entities
    within the same body,
  • 9:05 - 9:09
    and one decides to turn left,
    but the other goes right;
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    we would have trouble walking!
  • 9:11 - 9:15
    Yet, in some extreme cases,
  • 9:15 - 9:18
    this can happen.
  • 9:18 - 9:22
    Our consciousness
    can be split experimentally,
  • 9:22 - 9:26
    as we’ve observed in split-brain persons.
  • 9:26 - 9:30
    Roger Sperry was one of the first
    to do research in the field,
  • 9:30 - 9:35
    winning a Nobel prize for it
    about 30 years ago.
  • 9:35 - 9:36
    What happens to split-brain people?
  • 9:36 - 9:40
    They have their corpus callosum severed.
  • 9:40 - 9:43
    This disrupts the major connection
    between their two hemispheres,
  • 9:43 - 9:47
    which carries neural signals
  • 9:47 - 9:50
    between the two halves of the brain,
  • 9:50 - 9:56
    which allows this integration
    of information between them.
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    After this corpus callosum is severed,
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    which usually happens in treatment
    of severe epilepsy,
  • 10:00 - 10:04
    the patient appears to be
    a single human being
  • 10:05 - 10:07
    for most of the time.
  • 10:07 - 10:11
    Actually, in some specific conditions
  • 10:11 - 10:12
    which can be created,
  • 10:12 - 10:15
    we can observe the presence
    of two reasoning entities.
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    Basically, the patient has a barrier
  • 10:18 - 10:22
    positioned between the two visual fields.
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    As you probably know,
  • 10:24 - 10:28
    the right visual field
    maps onto the left hemisphere
  • 10:28 - 10:29
    and vice versa.
  • 10:30 - 10:34
    Then they're given some sight
    or voice-related instructions,
  • 10:34 - 10:39
    "According to the word
    you’ll see in front of you,
  • 10:39 - 10:45
    you shall either say it aloud
  • 10:45 - 10:48
    or take the corresponding object."
  • 10:49 - 10:54
    The left half of the brain,
    which is linked to the right visual field,
  • 10:54 - 10:56
    sees the word "RING".
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    Instead, the right hemisphere,
  • 10:59 - 11:03
    which is responsible
    for language and speech,
  • 11:03 - 11:06
    thinks: "I see the word RING,"
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    causing the patient to say: "Ring".
  • 11:09 - 11:10
    Simultaneously,
  • 11:10 - 11:14
    the right half of the brain
    sees the word "KEY"
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    but can't speak it, not having
    any concept of verbalization,
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    so the patient takes the key in his hand.
  • 11:19 - 11:20
    We have an individual
  • 11:20 - 11:23
    who takes the key
    and says "Ring" at the same time.
  • 11:23 - 11:27
    This demonstrates how integration
  • 11:27 - 11:30
    is crucial to the development
    of cognizant behaviour,
  • 11:30 - 11:32
    and how, in some cases,
    it can be interrupted.
  • 11:35 - 11:39
    Besides integration,
    a great ability to remap is needed.
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    That is, to constantly reorganize
  • 11:42 - 11:47
    what’s happening within the brain.
  • 11:47 - 11:52
    Basically, remapping means
    taking what is received
  • 11:52 - 11:54
    from the external world,
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    in this case, through the eyes
  • 11:57 - 12:02
    that detect the light reflected by objects
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    onto the retina,
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    and combining it
  • 12:06 - 12:09
    with our relevant value systems.
  • 12:09 - 12:13
    This is continuously carried out
  • 12:13 - 12:17
    on past perceptions,
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    up to a few seconds in the past,
  • 12:19 - 12:23
    and constantly updated as time passes.
  • 12:23 - 12:27
    We remember and as we move on,
    this changes our perspective.
  • 12:27 - 12:31
    It’s as if the back of the brain,
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    more concerned with raw signals processing
  • 12:34 - 12:38
    and feelings from the outside,
  • 12:38 - 12:45
    were constantly telling the front
    what’s going on around it.
  • 12:46 - 12:50
    The front then tells the back
  • 12:50 - 12:54
    how it must interpret those data,
    according to what it remembers.
  • 12:54 - 12:57
    That’s what Edelman calls
    "remembered present",
  • 12:57 - 13:01
    a continuous remapping, a dynamic process
  • 13:01 - 13:05
    that seems to underlie
    the most basic form of consciousness.
  • 13:05 - 13:08
    We're always speaking in the third person.
  • 13:09 - 13:12
    The last ingredient is narration ability.
  • 13:12 - 13:18
    This is the ability
    to remap this representation
  • 13:18 - 13:21
    of what’s going on and what I'm doing.
  • 13:21 - 13:25
    Or rather, what’s going on
    and what the brain is doing
  • 13:25 - 13:27
    in a self-centered perspective,
  • 13:27 - 13:30
    according to an intuition of the self
  • 13:30 - 13:34
    that’s determined mainly
    by the fact that we have one body
  • 13:34 - 13:37
    and that everything our brain perceives
  • 13:37 - 13:42
    is connected to its sensors and actuators,
  • 13:42 - 13:44
    which belong to our body.
  • 13:44 - 13:45
    Hence, this narration,
  • 13:45 - 13:50
    that is, this remapping,
    in a self-centered perspective,
  • 13:50 - 13:51
    happens over time.
  • 13:51 - 13:56
    In this system, actually there is
    no entity watching what’s going on.
  • 13:56 - 13:59
    There’s just a process,
  • 13:59 - 14:04
    chatting with itself
    about the meaning of the situations
  • 14:04 - 14:08
    in which it is.
  • 14:08 - 14:13
    From here, it can start building
    abstract and symbolic representations
  • 14:13 - 14:18
    ultimately creating
    both symbolic concepts of situations
  • 14:18 - 14:20
    and a self-concept,
  • 14:20 - 14:25
    a definition of its own identity.
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    Now you should have a basic idea,
  • 14:28 - 14:31
    not necessarily of what this means,
    because it’s a subjective experience,
  • 14:31 - 14:35
    but rather of what physical ingredients
  • 14:35 - 14:37
    are needed for this process to take place.
  • 14:37 - 14:41
    And in particular,
    of the activities and relationship
  • 14:41 - 14:43
    between the rest of the brain
  • 14:43 - 14:47
    and the prefrontal cortex.
  • 14:49 - 14:53
    If we consider various extreme cases
  • 14:53 - 14:57
    when conscience is dramatically inhibited,
  • 14:57 - 14:59
    if not suppressed,
  • 14:59 - 15:03
    such as coma, vegetative state,
  • 15:03 - 15:07
    the more common deep sleep,
  • 15:07 - 15:11
    or also general anaesthesia,
  • 15:11 - 15:13
    we see that, in all cases,
  • 15:13 - 15:17
    their common denominator is hypoactivity,
  • 15:17 - 15:21
    that is, reduced activity
    of the prefrontal regions
  • 15:21 - 15:26
    compared to the pattern of activity
    that can be observed in normal people
  • 15:26 - 15:31
    who are able to verbally give proof
    of their consciousness.
  • 15:34 - 15:38
    Finally, it is no coincidence
    that consciousness and self-awareness
  • 15:39 - 15:44
    seem to arise almost exclusively
  • 15:44 - 15:46
    in social animals.
  • 15:46 - 15:51
    A key factor in their development
  • 15:51 - 15:53
    and the discovery of their self-identity
  • 15:53 - 15:56
    is the ability to distinguish
    between self and non-self.
  • 15:56 - 15:58
    This learning process
  • 15:58 - 16:01
    is greatly stimulated
  • 16:01 - 16:06
    by interaction with other members
    of the same species.
  • 16:06 - 16:10
    There’s a very restricted club of animals,
  • 16:10 - 16:15
    which includes great apes,
    humans in particular,
  • 16:15 - 16:19
    and was recently joined also by elephants,
  • 16:19 - 16:23
    that are able to pass
    the so-called mirror test.
  • 16:23 - 16:26
    When in front of a mirror,
  • 16:26 - 16:29
    they understand that
    what they see is a reflection
  • 16:29 - 16:31
    and not another animal.
  • 16:31 - 16:33
    Also, if we paint a cross
    on the forehead of an elephant
  • 16:33 - 16:37
    while it’s sleeping,
    once in front of the mirror,
  • 16:37 - 16:41
    it will try to erase
    that cross with its trunk.
  • 16:41 - 16:44
    Other animals will simply try
    to interact with the mirror.
  • 16:46 - 16:50
    So, self-awareness is
    an individual perception.
  • 16:50 - 16:54
    Yet it’s a social sense,
    which arises in society.
  • 16:54 - 16:56
    But now, let’s go back to the Internet.
  • 16:58 - 17:02
    The Internet has developed
    its infrastructures,
  • 17:02 - 17:06
    its ways to obtain information
  • 17:06 - 17:10
    and the ability to influence
    the external world
  • 17:10 - 17:12
    on a massive scale.
  • 17:13 - 17:17
    This is particularly true
    for social networks.
  • 17:17 - 17:19
    These are constantly reorganizing
  • 17:19 - 17:21
    and operating an actual rewiring
  • 17:21 - 17:23
    of the social graph
    of connections between people,
  • 17:23 - 17:27
    affecting the way information spreads
  • 17:27 - 17:32
    among the nodes of the network,
    which ultimately represent ourselves.
  • 17:34 - 17:38
    As for the values, these were designed
  • 17:38 - 17:44
    and embedded into the system
    by its founding fathers.
  • 17:44 - 17:47
    Such as end-to-end equivalence,
  • 17:47 - 17:51
    which means that there are
    no special, differentiated nodes
  • 17:51 - 17:54
    and all the processing
    is done by the end nodes:
  • 17:54 - 17:55
    the one that sends
  • 17:55 - 17:58
    and the one that receives signals
    over the network.
  • 17:58 - 18:02
    It’s either an open system
    or a federal one.
  • 18:02 - 18:06
    On top of these principles,
    acquired values appear,
  • 18:06 - 18:12
    based on the constant feedbacks
  • 18:12 - 18:15
    that users and participants
    to this network
  • 18:15 - 18:20
    give about any bit of information
    that is uploaded on the Web.
  • 18:20 - 18:25
    They do it through systems like those
    that you see on any website these days,
  • 18:25 - 18:27
    which let you share a piece of content,
  • 18:27 - 18:29
    rate it or leave a comment.
  • 18:29 - 18:32
    These are nothing more
    than value judgements
  • 18:32 - 18:35
    that collectively contribute
  • 18:35 - 18:39
    to the organization of content
  • 18:39 - 18:43
    and information circulating on the Web.
  • 18:45 - 18:51
    Digging deeper, we discover
  • 18:51 - 18:53
    that there are also integration processes,
  • 18:53 - 18:57
    which can be monitored and tracked.
  • 18:57 - 19:00
    For example, the so-called
    information cascading processes,
  • 19:00 - 19:03
    according to which the probability
  • 19:03 - 19:06
    that I share a piece of information
  • 19:06 - 19:09
    is directly proportional
  • 19:09 - 19:14
    to the number of people
    that have already done so.
  • 19:14 - 19:17
    Such cascading processes,
  • 19:17 - 19:23
    which can cause
    some specific bits of content
  • 19:23 - 19:26
    to rapidly go viral at some point in time,
  • 19:26 - 19:27
    are the basis of phenomena
  • 19:27 - 19:31
    like the one that occurred
    during the elections in Iran.
  • 19:31 - 19:38
    Here, the popular revolt
    first broke out on the Internet.
  • 19:38 - 19:39
    Why?
  • 19:39 - 19:43
    Because there was no general perception
    of the level of dissent.
  • 19:43 - 19:46
    Only thanks to this way of gathering data,
  • 19:46 - 19:50
    more and more people started to realize
  • 19:50 - 19:52
    how widespread dissent was
  • 19:52 - 19:56
    and this ended up in massive protests.
  • 19:58 - 20:00
    Finally, we are witnessing
    the development of an ability
  • 20:00 - 20:05
    to classif,y in greater
    and greater detail, any information
  • 20:05 - 20:10
    that is uploaded or conveyed by the Web.
  • 20:10 - 20:14
    This is thanks to these systems
    of relationships among people,
  • 20:14 - 20:18
    virtual and real objects,
  • 20:18 - 20:24
    which are constantly reclassifying
    everything on the Internet.
  • 20:25 - 20:32
    Yet, we are still missing
    the narrating self.
  • 20:32 - 20:36
    This system is still trapped
    in the present.
  • 20:36 - 20:42
    There are no processes
    to analyze data over time,
  • 20:42 - 20:46
    which would enable the Web
    to take decisions and predict the future.
  • 20:47 - 20:49
    Yet, some people are working on it.
  • 20:49 - 20:52
    These are people that are used
  • 20:52 - 20:57
    to deal with and organize
    massive amounts of data.
  • 20:57 - 21:04
    So, to summarise, I don’t think
    the Internet is conscious.
  • 21:04 - 21:09
    It’s more likely to develop
    some forms of precognition
  • 21:09 - 21:12
    or collective organization,
  • 21:12 - 21:15
    which will be able to predict
  • 21:15 - 21:19
    and influence the behaviour
    of large sections of the population.
  • 21:19 - 21:21
    There will probably exist more than one.
  • 21:21 - 21:27
    And they will be capable
    of having a dramatic impact
  • 21:27 - 21:29
    on our civilization.
  • 21:29 - 21:36
    I believe that any piece of information
    created across the world
  • 21:36 - 21:38
    will end up on the Web in some way.
  • 21:38 - 21:41
    This is getting increasingly smarter.
  • 21:41 - 21:47
    It is getting less limited
    by data and time
  • 21:47 - 21:49
    in its processing of information.
  • 21:49 - 21:52
    There won’t be just one
    but rather several collective processes.
  • 21:53 - 21:56
    And they will redefine
  • 21:56 - 22:02
    our concept of collective conscience
  • 22:02 - 22:04
    and what we mean by it.
  • 22:04 - 22:08
    I guess some of these processes
    are already taking place.
  • 22:09 - 22:11
    Thank you for your attention.
  • 22:11 - 22:15
    (Applause)
Title:
Complexity | Eric Lumer | TEDxLakeComo
Description:

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community.

Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
Italian
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
22:17

English subtitles

Revisions