< Return to Video

What happens in your brain when you pay attention?

  • 0:01 - 0:03
    Paying close attention to something:
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    Not that easy, is it?
  • 0:06 - 0:11
    It's because our attention is pulled
    in so many different directions at a time,
  • 0:11 - 0:15
    and it's in fact pretty impressive
    if you can stay focused.
  • 0:16 - 0:20
    Many people think that attention
    is all about what we are focusing on,
  • 0:20 - 0:25
    but it's also about what information
    our brain is trying to filter out.
  • 0:26 - 0:29
    There are two ways
    you direct your attention.
  • 0:30 - 0:31
    First, there's overt attention.
  • 0:32 - 0:36
    In overt attention,
    you move your eyes towards something
  • 0:36 - 0:37
    in order to pay attention to it.
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    Then there's covert attention.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    In covert attention,
    you pay attention to something,
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    but without moving your eyes.
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    Think of driving for a second.
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    Your overt attention,
    your direction of the eyes,
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    are in front,
  • 0:56 - 0:57
    but that's your covert attention
  • 0:57 - 1:01
    which is constantly scanning
    the surrounding area,
  • 1:02 - 1:03
    where you don't actually look at them.
  • 1:06 - 1:07
    I'm a computational neuroscientist,
  • 1:07 - 1:11
    and I work on cognitive
    brain-machine interfaces,
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    or bringing together
    the brain and the computer.
  • 1:15 - 1:16
    I love brain patterns.
  • 1:17 - 1:18
    Brain patterns are important for us
  • 1:18 - 1:22
    because based on them
    we can build models for the computers,
  • 1:22 - 1:23
    and based on these models
  • 1:23 - 1:28
    computers can recognize
    how well our brain functions.
  • 1:28 - 1:29
    And if it doesn't function well,
  • 1:30 - 1:34
    then these computers themselves
    can be used as assistive devices
  • 1:35 - 1:36
    for therapies.
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    But that also means something,
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    because choosing the wrong patterns
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    will give us the wrong models
  • 1:44 - 1:45
    and therefore the wrong therapies.
  • 1:45 - 1:47
    Right?
  • 1:48 - 1:49
    In case of attention,
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    the fact that we can
  • 1:52 - 1:55
    shift our attention not only by our eyes
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    but also by thinking --
  • 1:57 - 2:02
    that makes covert attention
    an interesting model for computers.
  • 2:02 - 2:06
    So I wanted to know
    what are the brainwave patterns
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    when you look overtly
    or when you look covertly.
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    I set up an experiment for that.
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    In this experiment
    there are two flickering squares,
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    one of them flickering
    at a slower rate than the other one.
  • 2:21 - 2:24
    Depending on which of these flickers
    you are paying attention to,
  • 2:24 - 2:28
    certain parts of your brain
    will start resonating in the same rate
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    as that flickering rate.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    So by analyzing your brain signals,
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    we can track where exactly
    you are watching
  • 2:39 - 2:40
    or you are paying attention to.
  • 2:43 - 2:47
    So to see what happens in your brain
    when you pay overt attention,
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    I asked people to look directly
    in one of the squares
  • 2:51 - 2:52
    and pay attention to it.
  • 2:53 - 2:58
    In this case, not surprisingly,
    we saw that these flickering squares
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    appeared in their brain signals
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    which was coming
    from the back of their head,
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    which is responsible for the processing
    of your visual information.
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    But I was really interested
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    to see what happens in your brain
    when you pay covert attention.
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    So this time I asked people
    to look in the middle of the screen
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    and without moving their eyes,
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    to pay attention
    to either of these squares.
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    When we did that,
  • 3:27 - 3:31
    we saw that both of these flickering rates
    appeared in their brain signals,
  • 3:31 - 3:32
    but interestingly,
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    only one of them,
    which was paid attention to,
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    had stronger signals,
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    so there was something in the brain
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    which was handling this information
  • 3:43 - 3:49
    so that thing in the brain was basically
    the activation of the frontal area.
  • 3:50 - 3:53
    The front part of your brain
    is responsible
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    for higher cognitive functions as a human.
  • 3:57 - 4:02
    The frontal part,
    it seems that it works as a filter
  • 4:03 - 4:07
    trying to let information come in
    only from the right flicker
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    that you are paying attention to
  • 4:09 - 4:13
    and trying to inhibit the information
    coming from the ignored one.
  • 4:15 - 4:21
    The filtering ability of the brain
    is indeed a key for attention,
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    which is missing in some people,
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    for example in people with ADHD.
  • 4:27 - 4:32
    So a person with ADHD
    cannot inhibit these distractors,
  • 4:32 - 4:36
    and that's why they can't focus
    for a long time on a single task.
  • 4:38 - 4:39
    But what if this person
  • 4:39 - 4:43
    could play a specific computer game
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    with his brain connected to the computer,
  • 4:46 - 4:49
    and then train his own brain
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    to inhibit these distractors?
  • 4:54 - 4:56
    Well, ADHD is just one example.
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    We can use these cognitive
    brain-machine interfaces
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    for many other cognitive fields.
  • 5:04 - 5:06
    It was just a few years ago
  • 5:06 - 5:11
    that my grandfather had a stroke,
    and he lost complete ability to speak.
  • 5:13 - 5:16
    He could understand everybody,
    but there was no way to respond,
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    even not writing
    because he was illiterate.
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    So he passed away in silence.
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    I remember thinking at that time:
  • 5:27 - 5:31
    What if we could have a computer
  • 5:31 - 5:32
    which could speak for him?
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    Now, after years that I am in this field,
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    I can see that this might be possible.
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    Imagine if we can find brainwave patterns
  • 5:43 - 5:47
    when people think
    about images or even letters,
  • 5:48 - 5:51
    like the letter A generates
    a different brainwave pattern
  • 5:51 - 5:52
    than the letter B, and so on.
  • 5:53 - 5:57
    Could a computer one day
    communicate for people who can't speak?
  • 5:58 - 5:59
    What if a computer
  • 6:00 - 6:05
    can help us understand
    the thoughts of a person in a coma?
  • 6:06 - 6:07
    We are not there yet,
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    but pay close attention.
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    We will be there soon.
  • 6:12 - 6:13
    Thank you.
  • 6:13 - 6:19
    (Applause)
Title:
What happens in your brain when you pay attention?
Speaker:
Mehdi Ordikhani-Seyedlar
Description:

Attention isn't just about what we focus on -- it's also about what our brains filter out. By investigating patterns in the brain as people try to focus, computational neuroscientist Mehdi Ordikhani-Seyedlar hopes to bring the brain and the computer closer together, building models that can be used to treat ADHD and help those who have lost the ability to communicate. Hear more about this exciting science in this brief, fascinating talk.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
06:32

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions