-
[narrator] Children at play with
one of their favorite activities.
-
The water provides plenty of
opportunities for learning and fun.
-
As they grow up and take part in
activities like these,
-
one of the things that children
need to be able to do
-
is represent internally the
complexity of their social worlds.
-
This involves not just being able to
predict how the world of objects works,
-
but also how other people are likely
to react in various situations.
-
Psychologists have recently realized that,
in order to do this,
-
the young child needs an idea about
how people's minds work—
-
what they have begun to call
a "theory of mind."
-
And research over the last decade
-
has suggested how this seems to
appear quite rapidly
-
and suddenly between the ages of
three and four years.
-
The great thing about
theory of mind is that it...
-
it is a major conceptual tool
that allows us to step
-
outside of the bounds of
the directly-perceivable
-
stimulus circumstances,
and allow for
-
for predicting and explaining
other people's, uh, behavior
-
in a wide variety of novel ways.
-
[narrator] Psychologists argue that
we can see the difference between
-
having and not having a theory of mind
in simple experiments
-
called "false belief tasks."
-
First of all, the child has to establish
the fact that their own belief is false.
-
Then they have to put themselves
inside the mind of another child
-
in order to predict that child's beliefs.
-
[man] Now, Elizabeth,
what do you think's inside this box?
-
- Smarties.
- Have a look inside.
-
- Can't take the lid off.
- Oh, well, let me have a go, shall I?
-
You ready?
-
Say "go."
-
Go.
-
What's in there?
-
What's that?
- Pencils.
-
- Oh, right. Should we
put them back in the box?
-
- Yes.
But what are they for?
-
- Shall we put the lid on?
-
Now, can you remember
what's inside the box?
-
- Pencils.
-
- And what did you think
was in the box?
-
- Pencils.
-
- Did you?
-
Now I'm going to show this box
to Jay in a minute.
-
What will Jay think is in the box?
-
- Pencils.
-
- You think?
Good.
-
After you've mopped up
the child's tears
-
because they expected to get sweets,
you, uh, ask them two questions.
-
One is a reality question to make sure
that they remember what's inside.
-
So "what is inside the box?"
-
And then you can ask them
one of two false belief questions.
-
One is about their own previous belief—
"What did you think was inside the box?"
-
And the second question is,
-
"Your friend John is gonna come
and look at the box in a minute.
-
"What will John think is in the box?"
-
[Charlie Lewis] Do you like that?
-
[narrator] Like many children
of this age,
-
when asked what
another child will think,
-
Elizabeth simply projects what
she now believes herself
-
about the tube of sweets.
-
She doesn't recognize that the
other child exists as an independent,
-
thinking being.
-
And presented with
another task of this kind,
-
she acts in much the same way.
-
- [Charlie Lewis] ...right now.
- [Elizabeth] She is?
-
- This is Sally...
-
and this is Anne.
All right?
-
Now, Sally comes in with a marble.
-
Sally puts her marble
into the basket and goes out.
-
Anne comes and gets the marble,
and she puts the marble in the box.
-
Here comes Sally! Here comes Sally!
-
Now, where will Sally
look for the marble?
-
- [Elizabeth] Oh!
Let's find it.
-
There you go, Sally!
-
- You got it for her!
Hooray!
-
- [narrator] But for
another child, Connor,
-
the outcome is somewhat different.
-
- [Charlie Lewis] Sally comes in
to find her marble.
-
Where will Sally look for her marble?
-
And where was the
marble in the beginning?
-
That's it. Good.
Shall we look for the marble now?
-
Where did you say the marble is?
-
Have a look.
-
That's it. Good!
-
The joy of this procedure
-
is that you can ask two control
questions to make sure they understand
-
what the task is about.
-
You can ask them a memory question,
-
which is where was the object
at the beginning.
-
And of course the answer to that
is at location A.
-
And you can ask them
a second reality question,
-
which is where is the object now,
which of course is location B.
-
- [narrator] To be able to work out
where Sally will think the marble is,
-
Connor has to be able to
simultaneously hold—
-
or in psychological terms,
mentally represent—
-
two distinct points of view in his mind.
-
In the research studies, most
three-year-olds seem unable to do this.
-
Before the age of
about four, uh,
-
children do not use the other person's
mental representations
-
as independent entities.
-
They do not realize that individuals
can act upon misrepresentation.
-
- [Charlie Lewis]
This is Sally....
-
- [narrator] But most four-year-olds get
the answers to these questions right.
-
But why?
-
What happens between the ages
of three and four?