Return to Video

Perspectives and Innovation

  • 0:00 - 0:04
    Hi. In this lecture we’re talking about problem solving.
  • 0:04 - 0:09
    And we’re talking about the role that diverse perspectives play in finding solutions to problems.
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    So when you think about a problem,
  • 0:11 - 0:13
    perspective is how you represent it.
  • 0:13 - 0:17
    So remember from the previous lecture, we talked about landscapes.
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    We talked about landscape being a way to represent
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    the solutions along this axis
  • 0:22 - 0:27
    and the value of the solutions as the height.
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    And so this is metaphorically a way to represent
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    how someone might think about solving a problem:
  • 0:33 - 0:37
    Finding high points on their landscape.
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    What we want to do is take this metaphor and formalize it
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    and part of the reason for this course is to get better logic,
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    [in order to] think through things in a clear way.
  • 0:45 - 0:49
    So I’m going to take this landscape metaphor and turn it into a formal model.
  • 0:49 - 0:50
    So how do we do it?
  • 0:50 - 0:55
    The first thing we do is we formally define what a perspective is.
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    So we speak math to metaphor.
  • 0:57 - 0:59
    So what a perspective is going to be is
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    it’s going to be a representation of all possible solutions.
  • 1:01 - 1:05
    So it’s some encoding of the set of possible solutions to the problem.
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    Once we have that encoding of the set of possible solutions,
  • 1:09 - 1:13
    then we can create our landscape by just assigning a value to each one of those solutions.
  • 1:13 - 1:16
    And that will give us a landscape picture like you saw before.
  • 1:16 - 1:20
    Now most of us are familiar with perspectives,
  • 1:20 - 1:21
    even though we don’t know it.
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    Let me give some examples.
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    Remember when we took seventh grade math?
  • 1:25 - 1:28
    We learned about how to represent a point, how to plot points.
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    And we typically learned two ways to do it.
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    The first way was Cartesian coordinates.
  • 1:33 - 1:35
    So given a point, we would represent it
  • 1:35 - 1:39
    by and an X and a Y value in space.
  • 1:39 - 1:40
    So, it might be five units,
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    this would be the point, let’s say (5, 2).
  • 1:42 - 1:46
    It’s five units in the X direction, two units in the Y direction.
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    But we also learned another way to represent points,
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    and that was [polar] coordinates.
  • 1:51 - 1:52
    So we can take the same point and say,
  • 1:52 - 1:55
    there’s a radius, which is its distance from the origin,
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    and then there’s some angle theta,
  • 1:57 - 1:58
    which says how far we have to sweep out,
  • 1:58 - 2:03
    in order to sweep that radius out in order to get to the point.
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    So two totally reasonable ways to represent a point:
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    X and Y, R and theta.
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    Cartesian, polar.
  • 2:10 - 2:11
    Which is better?
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    Well, the answer? It depends.
  • 2:13 - 2:14
    Let me show you why.
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    Suppose I wanted to describe this line.
  • 2:16 - 2:20
    In order to describe this line I should use Cartesian coordinates,
  • 2:20 - 2:24
    ’cause I can just say Y=3 and X moves from two to five.
  • 2:24 - 2:25
    It’s really easy.
  • 2:25 - 2:29
    But suppose I wanna describe this arc.
  • 2:29 - 2:30
    If I wanna describe this arc,
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    now Cartesian coordinates are gonna be fairly complicated,
  • 2:33 - 2:35
    and I’d be better off using polar coordinates,
  • 2:35 - 2:36
    because the radius is fixed
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    and I just talked about how the radius is—you know,
  • 2:39 - 2:40
    there’s this distance R,
  • 2:40 - 2:43
    and theta just moves from, you know, A to B, let’s say.
  • 2:43 - 2:45
    So depending on what I want to do.
  • 2:45 - 2:47
    If I want to look at straight lines, I should use Cartesian.
  • 2:47 - 2:50
    And if I want to look at arcs, I should probably use polar.
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    So, perspectives depend on the problem.
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    Now let’s think about where we want to go.
  • 2:55 - 2:59
    We want to talk about how perspectives help us find solutions to problems
  • 2:59 - 3:02
    and how perspectives help us be innovative.
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    Well, if you look at the history of science a lot of great breakthroughs—
  • 3:04 - 3:06
    you know, we think about Newton,
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    you know, his theory of gravity—
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    you can think about people actually having new perspectives on old problems.
  • 3:11 - 3:13
    Let’s take an example.
  • 3:13 - 3:17
    So, Mendeleev came up with the periodic table,
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    and in the periodic table he represents the elements by atomic weight.
  • 3:20 - 3:22
    He’s got them in these different columns.
  • 3:22 - 3:26
    In doing so, by organizing the elements by atomic weight
  • 3:26 - 3:28
    he found all sorts of structure.
  • 3:28 - 3:31
    So all the metals line one column, stuff like that.
  • 3:31 - 3:33
    Remember—from high school chemistry class.
  • 3:33 - 3:37
    That’s a perspective: It’s a representation of a set of possible elements.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    He could’ve organized them alphabetically.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    But that wouldn’t have made much sense.
  • 3:41 - 3:45
    So alphabetic representation wouldn’t give us any structure.
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    Atomic weight representation gives us a lot of structure.
  • 3:47 - 3:51
    In fact, when Mendeleev wrote down
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    all the elements that were around at the time according to atomic weight,
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    there were gaps in his representation.
  • 3:57 - 3:59
    There were holes for elements that were missing.
  • 3:59 - 4:02
    Those elements became scandium, gallium, and germanium.
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    They were eventually found ten to fifteen years later,
  • 4:05 - 4:06
    after he’d written down the periodic table:
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    People went out and were able to find the missing elements.
  • 4:09 - 4:11
    That perspective, atomic weight,
  • 4:11 - 4:16
    ended up being a very useful way to organize our thinking about the elements.
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    We do it all the time now.
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    When you have any sort of task,
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    you’ll find that you’re actually using some sort of perspective.
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    Suppose that you’re hiring someone.
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    And you’ve got a bunch of recent college graduates who apply for a job.
  • 4:28 - 4:30
    And you’ve gotta think,
  • 4:30 - 4:32
    “Okay, how do I organize all these applicants?”
  • 4:32 - 4:34
    Let’s say 500 applicants.
  • 4:34 - 4:37
    One thing you could do is you could organize them by GPA:
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    Take the highest GPA down to the lowest GPA.
  • 4:40 - 4:41
    That’s be one representation.
  • 4:41 - 4:45
    And you might do that if you valued competence or achievement.
  • 4:45 - 4:48
    But you might also value work ethic.
  • 4:48 - 4:49
    And if that were the case you might instead organize
  • 4:49 - 4:53
    those same CV’s or application files by how thick they are.
  • 4:53 - 4:56
    [Those who’re going to do the] really thick ones are people who work really, really hard.
  • 4:56 - 4:58
    They’ve accomplished a lot.
  • 4:58 - 5:01
    Well, the third thing you might do is you might value creativity.
  • 5:01 - 5:02
    And you might say,
  • 5:02 - 5:05
    “Well, let’s put the ones that are sort of most colorful, most interesting over here.
  • 5:05 - 5:08
    And the ones that are least colorful and least interesting over here.”
  • 5:08 - 5:10
    That’s the third way to do it.
  • 5:10 - 5:12
    Now depending on what you’re hiring for,
  • 5:12 - 5:13
    depending on who the applicants are,
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    any one of these might be fine.
  • 5:15 - 5:20
    The only point I’m trying to make here is that there’s different ways to organize these applicants.
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    In each one of those ways you organize—
  • 5:22 - 5:23
    whether it’s in your head,
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    or whether it’s formally laying them out in some way—
  • 5:26 - 5:27
    is a perspective.
  • 5:27 - 5:32
    And those perspectives will determine how hard the problem will be for you.
  • 5:32 - 5:33
    Let me explain why.
  • 5:33 - 5:36
    Now I want to go back to the landscape metaphor.
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    And when I think of that landscape as being rugged,
  • 5:38 - 5:43
    and by rugged I mean that it doesn’t look like a single peak,
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    that there’s lots of peaks on it.
  • 5:45 - 5:48
    And I want to formalize this notion of peaks.
  • 5:48 - 5:50
    And I do so as follows:
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    I’m going to define what I call a local optima.
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    A local optima is a point such that
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    if you look at the points on either side of it,
  • 5:58 - 5:59
    they’re lower in value.
  • 5:59 - 6:02
    So it’s sort of a point that locally is the highest possible value.
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    So if I look at this particular rugged landscape again,
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    there’s three local optima: 1, 2, 3.
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    At any one of these three points, I’d be stuck:
  • 6:10 - 6:13
    If I looked to the left or to the right,
  • 6:13 - 6:15
    I wouldn’t find a solution that’s better.
  • 6:15 - 6:19
    So we think about what makes a good perspective:
  • 6:19 - 6:24
    A good perspective is going to be a perspective that doesn’t have many local optima.
  • 6:24 - 6:28
    A bad perspective is going to be one that has a lot of local optima.
  • 6:28 - 6:29
    Let me give you an example, okay?
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    So, suppose I’m coming up with a candy bar.
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    Suppose I’m tasked with coming up with a new candy bar.
  • 6:33 - 6:39
    So I have my team of chefs make a whole bunch of different confections for me to try,
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    and I want to find the very best one.
  • 6:41 - 6:44
    But there’re so many of them, there’s so many possibilities,
  • 6:44 - 6:45
    that I’m not even sure how to think about it.
  • 6:45 - 6:49
    But one way to represent those candy bars might be by the number of calories that they had.
  • 6:49 - 6:53
    So I can organize all the different things they make by number of calories.
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    And if I did that, maybe I’d have three local optima.
  • 6:56 - 7:00
    So that’s a reasonable way to represent these possible candy bars.
  • 7:01 - 7:03
    Alternatively, I might represent those candy bars
  • 7:03 - 7:06
    by masticity, which is chew time—
  • 7:06 - 7:07
    how long it takes to chew ’em.
  • 7:07 - 7:11
    So these would be the ones that maybe only take two minutes to chew.
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    And these may take twenty minutes to chew.
  • 7:13 - 7:17
    Well, chew time is probably not the best way to look at a candy bar.
  • 7:17 - 7:21
    And so, as a result, I’m going to have a landscape with many, many more peaks.
  • 7:22 - 7:25
    And so, because it’s got many more peaks, that’s more places I could get stuck.
  • 7:25 - 7:29
    So it’s not as good as a way to represent the possible solutions.
  • 7:29 - 7:31
    It’s not as good a perspective.
  • 7:31 - 7:36
    The best perspective would be what we call a Mount Fuji landscape,
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    the ideal landscape that just has one peak.
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    And these are called Mount Fuji landscapes
  • 7:40 - 7:41
    because if you’ve ever been to Japan,
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    and you look at Mount Fuji, it looks pretty much like this.
  • 7:43 - 7:45
    Actually not quite like this, there’s like snow on the top.
  • 7:45 - 7:48
    But for the most part, it looks just like one giant cone.
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    If you’re on a Mount Fuji landscape,
  • 7:50 - 7:51
    if you’re sitting at some point,
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    you can always just climb your way right up to the top.
  • 7:54 - 7:56
    So these single-peak landscapes are really good
  • 7:56 - 7:58
    because you’ve basically taken a problem
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    and made it very, very simple.
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    What would be an example of a Mount Fuji landscape?
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    I’m going to take a famous example.
  • 8:06 - 8:09
    So, a famous example comes from scientific management,
  • 8:09 - 8:10
    and due to Frederick Taylor.
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    Taylor famously solved for the optimal size of a shovel.
  • 8:12 - 8:15
    So let’s think about the shovel size landscape.
  • 8:15 - 8:18
    So, on this axis, I’ve got the size of the shovel.
  • 8:19 - 8:22
    And on this axis, I’ve got the value.
  • 8:22 - 8:23
    And what do I mean by the value?
  • 8:23 - 8:25
    I don’t mean how much I can sell the shovel for,
  • 8:25 - 8:27
    I mean it’s like how useful the shovel is at the task.
  • 8:27 - 8:29
    So let’s suppose we’re shoveling coal
  • 8:29 - 8:30
    and I want to think about
  • 8:30 - 8:33
    how many pounds of coal can some[one] shovel in a day
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    as a function of the size.
  • 8:35 - 8:38
    So let’s start out here where the size is zero.
  • 8:38 - 8:40
    So this is the size of the pan.
  • 8:40 - 8:42
    If I have a shovel has a pan of size zero,
  • 8:42 - 8:44
    that’s commonly known as a stick
  • 8:44 - 8:46
    and we can’t get anything.
  • 8:46 - 8:48
    We’re not going to shovel anything with a stick.
  • 8:48 - 8:50
    Well, if I make it bigger,
  • 8:50 - 8:52
    you know, make it the size of maybe like a little spoon or something,
  • 8:52 - 8:54
    then we can shovel a little bit.
  • 8:54 - 8:56
    And as I make the shovel bigger and bigger and bigger,
  • 8:56 - 8:59
    we, whoever, my workers, can shovel more and more coal.
  • 8:59 - 9:03
    But at some point, the shovel’s going to get a little bit too big.
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    And it’s going to be too heavy to lift.
  • 9:05 - 9:06
    And the worker’s going to get tired,
  • 9:06 - 9:07
    and I’ll shovel less,
  • 9:07 - 9:08
    he’ll shovel less and less and less and less.
  • 9:08 - 9:12
    And then eventually get to some point where the shovel’s so big
  • 9:12 - 9:14
    that he can’t even lift it,
  • 9:14 - 9:15
    and it’s as useless as the stick.
  • 9:15 - 9:21
    So if I look at value in terms of how much coal the person can shovel in a day is a function of the size of the shovel.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    I’m going to get a single-peaked landscape.
  • 9:23 - 9:25
    That’s going to be an easy problem to solve.
  • 9:25 - 9:30
    And this idea, that we could represent scientific problems in this way—
  • 9:30 - 9:34
    or we could put engineering problems in this way—and then climb our way to peaks,
  • 9:34 - 9:37
    is the basis is something called scientific management
  • 9:37 - 9:38
    And the idea was that you could then
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    by finding these high points on these landscapes,
  • 9:41 - 9:43
    find optimal solutions.
  • 9:43 - 9:46
    We’re only going to find out the optimal solution for sure
  • 9:46 - 9:48
    if your hill climbed like this—if it’s single peaked.
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    If it’s rugged and looks like this mess,
  • 9:51 - 9:52
    looks like Mount Fuji landscape you’re fine,
  • 9:52 - 9:53
    but if it looks like this mess, this masticity landscape,
  • 9:53 - 9:56
    if you have a bad perspective,
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    well then if you climbed hills
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    you could get stuck just about anywhere.
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    So what you’d like is you’d like a Mount Fuji landscape,
  • 10:04 - 10:08
    And in the case of simple things like this shovel, that’s easy to get.
  • 10:08 - 10:09
    Let me give you another example.
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    This one’s a lot of fun.
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    This is a favorite game of mine called Sum to fifteen
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    and was developed by Herb Simon
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    who’s a Nobel Prize winner in economics.
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    And Sum to fifteen was developed to show people
  • 10:20 - 10:23
    why diverse perspectives are so useful,
  • 10:23 - 10:25
    why different ways of representing a problem can make them easy,
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    can make them like Mount Fuji,
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    or can make them really difficult.
  • 10:29 - 10:31
    So here’s how Sum to fifteen works.
  • 10:31 - 10:35
    There’s cards numbered from one to nine face up on a table.
  • 10:35 - 10:37
    There’s nine cards in front of you.
  • 10:37 - 10:38
    There’s two players.
  • 10:38 - 10:42
    Each person.takes turns, taking a card.
  • 10:42 - 10:45
    until all the cards are gone, possibly—it could end sooner.
  • 10:45 - 10:50
    If anybody ever holds three cards that add up to exactly 15, they win.
  • 10:51 - 10:52
    That’s the game. So, really simple.
  • 10:52 - 10:54
    Nine cards. Alternate taking cards.
  • 10:54 - 10:58
    If you ever get exactly three that sum to fifteen you win.
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    So let me show you a game.
  • 11:00 - 11:02
    Here’s a game between two people,
  • 11:02 - 11:04
    [let’s] call them Paul and David.
  • 11:04 - 11:05
    Paul goes first. Now you’d think when you play this game
  • 11:05 - 11:08
    the thing to do would be to choose the five.
  • 11:08 - 11:12
    Paul chooses the four, which is sort of an odd choice.
  • 11:12 - 11:14
    David goes next so he takes the five.
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    Paul then takes the six.
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    Now the six is a strange choice
  • 11:19 - 11:23
    because four plus six plus five equals fifteen.
  • 11:23 - 11:26
    So it looks like there is no way that he can win.
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    Well this will be confusing to Doug.
  • 11:28 - 11:30
    So Doug’s going to take the eight.
  • 11:30 - 11:35
    Now notice eight plus five equals thirteen.
  • 11:35 - 11:38
    So that means Paul has to take the two.
  • 11:38 - 11:39
    So he takes the two.
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    Well think about what happens next:
  • 11:42 - 11:43
    Four plus two is six.
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    So if Doug doesn’t take the nine, he’s going to lose.
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    But six plus two is eight.
  • 11:48 - 11:50
    So if Doug doesn’t take the seven he’s going to lose.
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    So what you’ve got here is that Paul has won.
  • 11:52 - 11:55
    No matter what Doug does, Paul’s going to win the game.
  • 11:56 - 11:57
    Now this is a pretty tricky game, right?
  • 11:57 - 11:59
    It was developed by a Nobel Prize winner.
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    You could imagine there’s lots of strategy involved.
  • 12:01 - 12:06
    I want to show you this game in a different perspective.
  • 12:06 - 12:08
    Remember the magic square from seventh grade math?
  • 12:08 - 12:11
    Every row adds up to fifteen—
  • 12:11 - 12:16
    8+3+4, 1+5+9, 6+7+2 —
  • 12:16 - 12:17
    so does every column—
  • 12:17 - 12:20
    8+1+6 sums up to fifteen;
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    3+5+7 sums up to fifteen—
  • 12:23 - 12:25
    and even the diagonals—
  • 12:25 - 12:27
    eight, five, two is fifteen;
  • 12:27 - 12:28
    six, five, four is fifteen.
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    Every row, every column, every diagonal sum up to fifteen.
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    Let me show you this game again on the Magic Square.
  • 12:34 - 12:37
    So, it’s just a different perspective on “Sum to Fifteen”.
  • 12:37 - 12:40
    Paul goes first, and takes the four.
  • 12:40 - 12:42
    Doug goes next and takes the five.
  • 12:42 - 12:46
    Paul takes the six, which is an odd choice, because now he can’t win.
  • 12:46 - 12:50
    Doug then takes the eight, Paul blocks him with the two.
  • 12:50 - 12:55
    But now it turns out, either the nine or seven will let Paul win.
  • 12:55 - 12:58
    What game is this?
  • 12:58 - 13:01
    Well, you’re right, it’s tic-tac-toe.
  • 13:01 - 13:04
    Sum to fifteen is just tic-tac-toe,
  • 13:04 - 13:07
    but on a different perspective, using a different perspective.
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    So if you turn Sum to Fifteen—
  • 13:09 - 13:12
    if you moved the cards 1 to 9 and put them in the magic square—
  • 13:12 - 13:16
    what you do is you create a Mount Fuji landscape In a sense:
  • 13:16 - 13:19
    You make the problem really simple.
  • 13:19 - 13:20
    So a lot of great breakthroughs,
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    like the periodic table,
  • 13:22 - 13:23
    Newton’s Theory of Gravity,
  • 13:23 - 13:26
    those are perspectives on problems
  • 13:26 - 13:28
    that turned something that was really difficult to figure out
  • 13:28 - 13:31
    into something that suddenly makes a lot of sense,
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    very easy to see the solution.
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    At least it’s something I call in my book, one of my books,
    the difference,
  • 13:35 - 13:37
    I call this the Savant Existence Theorem.
  • 13:37 - 13:40
    For any problem that’s out there,
  • 13:40 - 13:42
    there exists some way to represent it,
  • 13:42 - 13:45
    so that you turn it into a Mt. Fuji problem.
  • 13:45 - 13:46
    Now, why is that?
  • 13:46 - 13:47
    Well, it’s actually fairly straightforward.
  • 13:47 - 13:50
    All you have to do is,
  • 13:50 - 13:53
    if you’ve got all the solutions here represented on this thing,
  • 13:53 - 13:55
    you put the very best one in the middle.
  • 13:55 - 13:57
    And then put the worst ones at the end.
  • 13:57 - 13:59
    And then just sort of line up the solutions in such a way
  • 13:59 - 14:01
    so that you turn it into a Mount Fuji.
  • 14:01 - 14:03
    So it’s very straightforward.
  • 14:03 - 14:04
    Now the thing is, in order to make the Mount Fuji,
  • 14:04 - 14:07
    you’d have to know the solution already.
  • 14:07 - 14:09
    This isn’t a good way to solve problems
  • 14:09 - 14:12
    but the point is, it exists.
  • 14:12 - 14:13
    So it’s always the possibility
  • 14:13 - 14:15
    that someone could look at particular problem and said,
  • 14:15 - 14:17
    “Hey, what if think of it this way?”
  • 14:17 - 14:20
    And doing so turn something that was really rugged
  • 14:20 - 14:23
    into something that looks like Mount Fuji.
  • 14:24 - 14:26
    Here is the flip side though.
  • 14:26 - 14:28
    There is a ton of bad perspectives.
  • 14:28 - 14:31
    So just like there’s these Mount Fuji perspectives,
  • 14:31 - 14:34
    there’s also lots and lots of horrible ways to look at problems.
  • 14:34 - 14:37
    Think about this: Suppose I have just ten alternatives
  • 14:37 - 14:40
    and I want to think about what are all the different ways I can just put them in a line.
  • 14:40 - 14:42
    Well there’s ten things I could put first,
  • 14:42 - 14:44
    nine things I could put second,
  • 14:44 - 14:46
    eight things I could put third and so on.
  • 14:46 - 14:51
    So there’s 10 × 9 × 8 × 7 × 6 × 5 × 4 × 3 × 2 × 1 perspectives.
  • 14:51 - 14:54
    Most of those are going to not be very good.
  • 14:54 - 14:58
    They’re not going to organize this set of solutions in any useful way.
  • 14:58 - 15:01
    Particularly, only a few of them are going to create Mount Fujis.
  • 15:01 - 15:04
    So we think about the value of perspectives, what we get is this:
  • 15:04 - 15:07
    There’s really good ones out there,
  • 15:07 - 15:10
    that insightful, smart people can come up
  • 15:10 - 15:12
    with really good representations of problem[s]
  • 15:12 - 15:14
    to make the landscapes less rugged.
  • 15:14 - 15:17
    If we just think about things in random ways,
  • 15:17 - 15:19
    we’re likely to get a landscape that’s so rugged
  • 15:19 - 15:21
    that we’re going to get stuck just about everywhere.
  • 15:21 - 15:23
    We’re not going to be able to find good solutions to the problem.
  • 15:23 - 15:27
    And we’re going to hit things that look like the masticity landscape,
  • 15:27 - 15:29
    and we’re going to get things with lots and lots of peaks.
  • 15:29 - 15:33
    Let’s move on now and talk about how we move on these landscapes.
  • 15:33 - 15:36
    So once I got our landscape, how do I find better solutions?
  • 15:36 - 15:39
    Are there other alternatives to just sort of climbing a hill?
  • 15:39 - 15:42
    Because that hill climbing idea really only works in one dimension.
  • 15:42 - 15:44
    What if I’ve got all sorts of dimensions?
  • 15:44 - 15:45
    How do I think about…
  • 15:46 - 15:47
    (Just a sec…)
  • 15:54 - 15:55
    So what have we learned?
  • 15:55 - 15:58
    First thing we’ve learned is that when we go about trying to solve a problem,
  • 15:58 - 16:00
    when we encode it in some way,
  • 16:00 - 16:02
    that’s a perspective.
  • 16:02 - 16:07
    And a perspective creates peaks; it creates these local optima.
  • 16:07 - 16:10
    So a better perspectives have fewer local optima.
  • 16:10 - 16:13
    Worse perspectives have lots of local optima.
  • 16:13 - 16:16
    And if you think about how many perspectives are out there,
  • 16:16 - 16:18
    we just saw there’s billions of them.
  • 16:18 - 16:19
    Because there’s billions of perspectives,
  • 16:19 - 16:21
    most of those probably aren’t very useful.
  • 16:21 - 16:25
    Some of them, though, turn problems into Mount Fujis.
  • 16:25 - 16:27
    And sometimes it takes a genius—
  • 16:27 - 16:29
    it takes a Newton, it takes a Mendeleev—
  • 16:29 - 16:31
    to come up with a way of representing reality
  • 16:31 - 16:33
    so that something that was incredibly rugged
  • 16:33 - 16:35
    becomes Mount Fuji–like.
  • 16:35 - 16:37
    Other times, if you think about the size of a shovel,
  • 16:37 - 16:42
    that problem most of us could probably figure out a way that problem just by shovel size,
  • 16:42 - 16:44
    so that it becomes a Mount Fuji.
  • 16:44 - 16:45
    The big point is this:
  • 16:45 - 16:49
    When we go about solving problems, the first thing we do is we encode them.
  • 16:49 - 16:51
    We have some representation of the problem.
  • 16:51 - 16:56
    That representation determines how hard the problem will be.
  • 16:56 - 16:58
    If we represent it in such a way that it’s a Mount Fuji, it’s easy.
  • 16:58 - 17:02
    If we represent it in such a way that it looks like that masticity landscape,
  • 17:02 - 17:04
    it’s probably going to be fairly hard.
  • 17:04 - 17:06
    Where we want to go next,
  • 17:06 - 17:10
    is we want to talk about once we’ve got this representation of the possible solutions,
  • 17:10 - 17:12
    once we have that landscape, so to speak,
  • 17:12 - 17:13
    how do we search on that landscape?
  • 17:13 - 17:15
    So one thing we’ve talked about was climbing hills.
  • 17:15 - 17:17
    But there’s lots of different ways you can climb hills.
  • 17:17 - 17:21
    That’s what we’ll talk about next: the heuristics we use on a landscape.
  • 17:21 -
    Thanks.
Title:
Perspectives and Innovation
Video Language:
English

English subtitles

Revisions