Return to Video

Lecture 2 - Segment 1

  • 0:00 - 0:07
    I’d like to follow up on our last lecture about the sociological imagination and talk about three questions
  • 0:07 - 0:11
    that are characteristic of the discipline of sociology.
  • 0:11 - 0:17
    And the first is: How are the things that we take to be natural socially constructed?
  • 0:17 - 0:22
    How are the things that we take to be natural socially constructed?
  • 0:22 - 0:30
    There’s a basic flaw in common sense, and human reasoning more generally, that goes something like this:
  • 0:30 - 0:37
    The things we see before us every day are “supposed to be that way”; they come from nature.
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    But sociology teaches us that many of the things
  • 0:40 - 0:46
    that we think are natural are actually man- and woman-made —
  • 0:46 - 0:51
    which does not necessarily mean this makes us freer from so-called “nature” —
  • 0:51 - 0:57
    in fact, we may not be as free as we think, even armed with this insight.
  • 0:57 - 1:05
    But that hasn’t stopped many people from using the insight to try to bring about very important social changes,
  • 1:05 - 1:11
    some of which have succeeded and some of which are slow to progress.
  • 1:11 - 1:15
    For example, let’s say that you go up to a baby or a small child on the street —
  • 1:15 - 1:22
    at least here in Princeton, New Jersey — perhaps in a stroller or crawling next to the parents.
  • 1:22 - 1:26
    What’s the first question you’re going to ask those parents?
  • 1:26 - 1:33
    Very likely you’re going to say, “Does your child have a penis or a vagina?”
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    You need to know the answer to that question right?
  • 1:36 - 1:44
    Because without knowing the answer to that question you cannot proceed any further in the interaction —
  • 1:44 - 1:48
    that is, if you are a typical human being, like me.
  • 1:48 - 1:53
    And this is because without knowing if the child has a penis or a vagina
  • 1:53 - 1:58
    you don’t really know how to interact with that child.
  • 1:58 - 2:05
    Now in fact, it’s probably not the best thing in the world to ask that question in that way to a parent.
  • 2:05 - 2:10
    In fact, if you were to go up and ask the question in that way,
  • 2:10 - 2:15
    at least here in Princeton, and all of the other places where I have lived,
  • 2:15 - 2:19
    you would be considered very unusual, if not strange.
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    So instead, we ask the question in a more benign way:
  • 2:23 - 2:29
    We say, “Is your child a boy, or a girl?”
  • 2:29 - 2:34
    Well, actually, even if you ask it like that, some parents will not be thrilled
  • 2:34 - 2:38
    because they want to believe that you can tell what their child is.
  • 2:38 - 2:45
    Which highlights how significant it is that we be able to interact with a child in an appropriate way.
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    So when you go up to a child on the street,
  • 2:48 - 2:53
    the first thing you might say to a parent is; “What’s your baby’s name?”
  • 2:53 - 2:58
    That’s a kind of subtle way of asking the sex of the boy or the girl
  • 2:58 - 3:04
    without saying “I can’t really tell if your child is a boy or a girl.”
  • 3:04 - 3:10
    And once you’ve figured out whether or not you are interacting with a boy or a girl,
  • 3:10 - 3:17
    that might cause some significant difference in the way that you will interact, or what you will say next.
  • 3:17 - 3:22
    So if you find out that it is a little boy named Michael — which is a popular boy’s name
  • 3:22 - 3:29
    when people currently at Princeton were born in the early 90’s —, you might say, “Hey buddy, how are you doing?”
  • 3:29 - 3:35
    Or if you find out that it’s a little girl named Ashley — which was also a popular girl’s name in the early 90’s,
  • 3:35 - 3:39
    when many of the students at Princeton who are here today were born —,
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    you might say, “Hi sweetie, how are you?”
  • 3:41 - 3:47
    That will be the beginning of a kind of interaction that is gender-based.
  • 3:47 - 3:52
    By gender, we mean The social, cultural and psychological meanings
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    which get attributed to sex.
  • 3:55 - 4:02
    And I’d be curious, by the way, about how this all works in the places that you live.
  • 4:02 - 4:07
    Why is gender so important to social interaction?
  • 4:07 - 4:09
    There’s nothing else quite like that.
  • 4:09 - 4:14
    If we go up to a baby on the street, and we don’t know what race they are,
  • 4:14 - 4:19
    we can pretty well interact with them, at least here in the United States.
  • 4:19 - 4:25
    Sure, there are parents who will signal a certain racial affiliation by how they fix the child’s hair,
  • 4:25 - 4:28
    or what kind of garments they have the child wear;
  • 4:28 - 4:35
    but on the whole, people don’t need to know the race of a child in order to interact with it.
  • 4:35 - 4:40
    Whether the child is black or white or Latino or Asian for example,
  • 4:40 - 4:45
    is not going to have a significant impact on the nature of the interaction.
  • 4:45 - 4:48
    The same thing with social class, right?
  • 4:48 - 4:55
    We don’t really need to know what social class a child comes from in order to interact with it.
  • 4:55 - 5:00
    In order for an interaction to be successful, we don’t need to know
  • 5:00 - 5:05
    whether a child is from the working classes or the middle classes or the upper middle classes
  • 5:05 - 5:10
    or that group that has become popularly known as the top one percent.
  • 5:10 - 5:14
    There are many of the people of the upper classes
  • 5:14 - 5:17
    who dress their children in clothes from the Gap or Old Navy,
  • 5:17 - 5:22
    stores which sell their products very widely to people of many different classes.
  • 5:22 - 5:25
    And likewise, there are many poor people in the United States
  • 5:25 - 5:32
    who dress their children in labels that come from elite names like Ralph Lauren or Tommy Hilfiger.
  • 5:32 - 5:36
    You can’t necessarily tell what social class a baby comes from.
  • 5:36 - 5:40
    And more importantly, you don’t feel you need to know.
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    But gender is completely different.
  • 5:43 - 5:48
    You expect to know the gender of a child before you can interact with it.
  • 5:48 - 5:54
    Now the one thing that occurs to you, or to me when you need to know the gender of a child
  • 5:54 - 6:01
    and that all interactions are gendered from the beginning is you come to the realization
  • 6:01 - 6:07
    that from a very early age a child is going to be enacting the role of a boy or a girl.
  • 6:07 - 6:14
    They re going to respond to the expectations of the people around them with regard to gender.
  • 6:14 - 6:21
    And through processes of interaction, they are going to come to think about themselves as a boy or a girl.
  • 6:21 - 6:27
    And because this happens at such a young age, when they’re infants, in fact,
  • 6:27 - 6:34
    we can see the ways that gender expectations come to be socially constructed at the earliest part of life.
  • 6:34 - 6:41
    Now this is not to suggest that this thing I am saying is social, does not have some biological basis to it.
  • 6:41 - 6:46
    This is not to suggest that brain science does not have a significant amount
  • 6:46 - 6:53
    to contribute to our understanding by looking at the differences between male and female brains.
  • 6:53 - 6:55
    But what it does suggest
  • 6:55 - 7:02
    is that there is a very strong component of male and female which is socially constructed.
  • 7:02 - 7:04
    Part of my agenda in this lecture is
  • 7:04 - 7:08
    not to say that the biological is a residual category that doesn’t matter,
  • 7:08 - 7:13
    but to say that we’re going to engage in an enterprise of disentangling,
  • 7:13 - 7:18
    to try to figure out which parts are biological, and which are social;
  • 7:18 - 7:22
    because from the standpoint of common sense, it is all biological.
  • 7:22 - 7:28
    Most people who are not educated in sociology see these differences as rooted in nature.
  • 7:28 - 7:35
    A few years ago, the president of Harvard, Larry Summers made a deliberately provocative statement —
  • 7:35 - 7:40
    for which he later apologized — suggesting that the existence of fewer women in science
  • 7:40 - 7:44
    might have something to do with there being fewer women
  • 7:44 - 7:50
    at the higher end of the intelligence distribution as measured by IQ scores.
  • 7:50 - 7:55
    He was trying to suggest that there was something innate or natural about this outcome.
  • 7:55 - 8:00
    Now, once we know that there is a strong social component to male and female,
  • 8:00 - 8:05
    we may hope that there are things that we can do to influence the environment
  • 8:05 - 8:09
    that may actually have an impact on the long-term outcomes of men and women.
  • 8:09 - 8:14
    We know that there is significant amount of gender inequality even in the United States today,
  • 8:14 - 8:20
    a country that has been thinking about these differences longer than some other countries.
  • 8:20 - 8:26
    And at the same time we find that many of these differences are intractable, or very difficult to eradicate.
  • 8:26 - 8:30
    But to come back to the statement by the president of Harvard,
  • 8:30 - 8:36
    there’s actually been some progress in getting more women into fields like biological sciences.
  • 8:36 - 8:39
    Summers’ remarks ignored some very important data.
  • 8:39 - 8:46
    In 1966, less than one percent of U.S. doctoral degrees in engineering were awarded to women;
  • 8:46 - 8:51
    while in 2001, the number had risen to about 17%.
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    Surely the IQ of women at the high end of the distribution
  • 8:54 - 8:58
    did not change that significantly during this period.
  • 8:58 - 9:01
    These numbers suggest that a lot of progress still needs to be made.
  • 9:01 - 9:06
    Some of our institutions in American society are working very hard
  • 9:06 - 9:10
    to ask what we can do to change gender inequality
  • 9:10 - 9:17
    and to change the sense of how it is natural for men and women to behave or act in particular ways.
  • 9:17 - 9:23
    In math and science training, a vast amount of sociological research has demonstrated
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    that when teachers or parents have low expectations for girls,
  • 9:26 - 9:30
    then women will not develop their potential talents.
  • 9:30 - 9:34
    We know that, for a very long time in universities like Princeton,
  • 9:34 - 9:38
    there were very few women who went into science and engineering.
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    And there was a sense on the part of many women
  • 9:40 - 9:45
    who went into these universities that it was a male thing to do.
  • 9:45 - 9:49
    This view was something encouraged at the earliest stage of life.
  • 9:49 - 9:53
    Over time expectations have changed,
  • 9:53 - 9:57
    and we have more and more women going into science and engineering.
  • 9:57 - 10:01
    But these are things that the university knows begin with a pipeline.
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    and they can only be changed if the stereotypes
  • 10:04 - 10:07
    and the sense of what is appropriate and natural for different genders to take on
  • 10:07 - 10:11
    changes at the earliest parts of life,
  • 10:11 - 10:18
    coinciding, perhaps, with the very moment when we are starting to interact with children in gendered ways.
  • 10:18 - 10:25
    Or let’s take an even more controversial example: racial differences in IQ.
  • 10:25 - 10:30
    Sometimes, when white people find out that I’m a sociologist
  • 10:30 - 10:35
    they’ll ask why it is that blacks are doing worse than whites in the United States.
  • 10:35 - 10:40
    And it’s not uncommon for folks to suggest to me that it is because they’re not as smart.
  • 10:40 - 10:45
    And they’ll cite evidence that blacks have lower IQ scores in general —
  • 10:45 - 10:49
    Quote, they’re not as smart so they don’t do as well, right?
  • 10:49 - 10:56
    Now it is true that blacks have lower IQ scores, in general, than whites in the United States.
  • 10:56 - 11:00
    People who argue this argue that it’s based on genetics.
  • 11:00 - 11:06
    One explanation is that they have smaller brains than whites; but that is wrong.
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    In his book « Intelligence and How to Get It », Richard Nesbitt says
  • 11:09 - 11:14
    that Albert Einstein had a smaller brain than the average black person —
  • 11:14 - 11:18
    Brain size is not the cause of these differences.
  • 11:18 - 11:22
    There’s also the claim that these lower IQ’s are inherited.
  • 11:22 - 11:26
    Well, there are many studies that have been done that have demonstrated
  • 11:26 - 11:32
    the ways that IQ is also based on the environmental influences — the social context in which people live.
  • 11:32 - 11:37
    Thus, according to Nisbett, the average child in a poor family will hear
  • 11:37 - 11:43
    substantially fewer words, per day, than someone in an upper middle class family.
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    By a time a child reaches five years old,
  • 11:45 - 11:50
    he or she will have heard many more words in a higher income family.
  • 11:50 - 11:57
    Vocabulary, it turns out, is a very significant determinant of how people do on IQ tests.
  • 11:57 - 12:01
    According to Nisbett, the average IQ in the United States
  • 12:01 - 12:07
    over the course of the past 50 years — since World War II — has gone up 15 to 20 points.
  • 12:07 - 12:10
    That is just the average for Americans as a whole.
  • 12:10 - 12:17
    And it’s impossible for the genetics of the country to have changed that much over the past 50 years.
  • 12:17 - 12:21
    So we know that there is something about the environment of the United States
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    that is affecting the group as a whole.
  • 12:24 - 12:30
    And during this period, the average difference in IQ between blacks and whites decreased significantly.
  • 12:30 - 12:35
    It was fifteen points in 1945 and it’s nine points today.
  • 12:35 - 12:40
    According to Nisbett, that corresponds to a certain level of improvement in the black population —
  • 12:40 - 12:44
    compared to the white population — in their standard of living.
  • 12:44 - 12:53
    The average black today has a higher IQ than the average white in 1950 — another interesting thing to explain.
  • 12:53 - 12:56
    So these are some examples of how it is
  • 12:56 - 13:01
    that one of the great achievements of the social sciences and thinking about IQ
  • 13:01 - 13:06
    has been to understand the way that many things we take to be natural
  • 13:06 - 13:12
    are not completely based on genetics, but are themselves a function of the social environment.
Title:
Lecture 2 - Segment 1
Video Language:
English
Ambrose LI edited English subtitles for Lecture 2 - Segment 1
Ambrose LI edited English subtitles for Lecture 2 - Segment 1
stanford-bot edited English subtitles for Lecture 2 - Segment 1
stanford-bot edited English subtitles for Lecture 2 - Segment 1
stanford-bot added a translation

English subtitles

Revisions