WEBVTT 00:00:00.200 --> 00:00:03.256 Greg Gage: If I asked you to think of a ferocious killer animal, 00:00:03.280 --> 00:00:04.776 you'd probably think of a lion, 00:00:04.800 --> 00:00:07.616 and for all the wonderful predatory skills that a lion has, 00:00:07.640 --> 00:00:11.336 it still only has about a 20 percent success rate at catching a meal. 00:00:11.360 --> 00:00:13.376 Now, one of the most successful hunters 00:00:13.400 --> 00:00:15.456 in the entire animal kingdom is surprising: 00:00:15.480 --> 00:00:16.736 the dragonfly. 00:00:16.760 --> 00:00:18.536 Now, dragonflies are killer flies, 00:00:18.560 --> 00:00:20.136 and when they see a smaller fly, 00:00:20.160 --> 00:00:23.136 they have about a 97 percent chance of catching it for a meal. 00:00:23.160 --> 00:00:24.416 And this is in mid-flight. 00:00:24.440 --> 00:00:27.616 But how can such a small insect be so precise? 00:00:27.640 --> 00:00:29.496 In this episode, we're going to see 00:00:29.520 --> 00:00:33.200 how the dragonfly's brain is highly specialized to be a deadly killer. NOTE Paragraph 00:00:33.224 --> 00:00:36.455 [DIY Neuroscience] NOTE Paragraph 00:00:36.480 --> 00:00:39.496 So what makes the dragonfly one of the most successful predators 00:00:39.520 --> 00:00:40.736 in the animal kingdom? 00:00:40.760 --> 00:00:42.016 One, it's the eyes. 00:00:42.040 --> 00:00:43.896 It has near 360-degree vision. 00:00:43.920 --> 00:00:45.336 Two, the wings. 00:00:45.360 --> 00:00:47.176 With individual control of its wings, 00:00:47.200 --> 00:00:49.576 the dragonfly can move precisely in any direction. 00:00:49.600 --> 00:00:51.976 But the real secret to the dragonfly's success 00:00:52.000 --> 00:00:55.096 is how its brain coordinates this complex information 00:00:55.120 --> 00:00:56.656 between the eyes and the wings 00:00:56.680 --> 00:00:59.256 and turns hunting into a simple reflex. 00:00:59.280 --> 00:01:01.661 To study this, Jaimie's been spending a lot of time 00:01:01.685 --> 00:01:03.336 socializing with dragonflies. 00:01:03.360 --> 00:01:05.296 What do you need to do your experiments? NOTE Paragraph 00:01:05.319 --> 00:01:07.656 Jaimie Spahr: First of all, you need dragonflies. NOTE Paragraph 00:01:07.680 --> 00:01:10.136 Oliver: I have a mesh cage to catch the dragonflies. NOTE Paragraph 00:01:10.160 --> 00:01:13.296 JS: The more I worked with them, the more terrified I got of them. 00:01:13.320 --> 00:01:16.096 They're actually very scary, especially under a microscope. 00:01:16.120 --> 00:01:19.256 They have really sharp mandibles, are generally pretty aggressive, 00:01:19.280 --> 00:01:22.016 which I guess also helps them to be really good predators. NOTE Paragraph 00:01:22.040 --> 00:01:25.176 GG: In order to learn what's going on inside the dragonfly's brain 00:01:25.200 --> 00:01:26.416 when it sees a prey, 00:01:26.440 --> 00:01:28.576 we're going to eavesdrop in on a conversation 00:01:28.600 --> 00:01:30.136 between the eyes and the wings, 00:01:30.160 --> 00:01:32.922 and to do that, we need to anesthetize the dragonfly on ice 00:01:32.946 --> 00:01:36.336 and make sure we protect its wings so that we can release it afterwards. 00:01:36.360 --> 00:01:40.016 Now, the dragonfly's brain is made up of specialized cells called neurons 00:01:40.040 --> 00:01:42.216 and these neurons are what allow the dragonfly 00:01:42.240 --> 00:01:43.896 to see and move so quickly. 00:01:43.920 --> 00:01:46.976 The individual neurons form circuits by connecting to each other 00:01:47.000 --> 00:01:49.616 via long, tiny threads called axons 00:01:49.640 --> 00:01:52.896 and the neurons communicate over these axons using electricity. 00:01:52.920 --> 00:01:56.376 In the dragonfly, we're going to place little metal wires, or electrodes, 00:01:56.400 --> 00:01:57.656 along the axon tracks, 00:01:57.680 --> 00:01:59.176 and this is what's really cool. 00:01:59.200 --> 00:02:01.216 In the dragonfly, there's only 16 neurons; 00:02:01.240 --> 00:02:02.496 that's eight per eye 00:02:02.520 --> 00:02:04.976 that tell the wings exactly where the target is. 00:02:05.000 --> 00:02:08.216 We've placed the electrodes so that we can record from these neurons 00:02:08.240 --> 00:02:09.936 that connect the eyes to the wings. 00:02:09.960 --> 00:02:12.816 Whenever a message is being passed from the eye to the wing, 00:02:12.840 --> 00:02:16.576 our electrode intercepts that conversation in the form of an electrical current, 00:02:16.600 --> 00:02:17.816 and it amplifies it. 00:02:17.840 --> 00:02:20.896 Now, we can both hear it and see it in the form of a spike, 00:02:20.920 --> 00:02:23.176 which we also call an action potential. NOTE Paragraph 00:02:23.200 --> 00:02:24.456 Now let's listen in. 00:02:24.480 --> 00:02:27.936 Right now, we have the dragonfly flipped upside down, 00:02:27.960 --> 00:02:30.576 so he's looking down towards the ground. 00:02:30.600 --> 00:02:33.576 We're going to take a prey, or what we sometimes call a target. 00:02:33.600 --> 00:02:36.096 In this case, the target's going to be a fake fly. 00:02:36.120 --> 00:02:38.640 We're going to move it into the dragonfly's sights. NOTE Paragraph 00:02:40.640 --> 00:02:42.296 (Buzzing) NOTE Paragraph 00:02:42.320 --> 00:02:43.520 Oh! 00:02:44.720 --> 00:02:45.920 Oh, look at that. 00:02:50.040 --> 00:02:52.240 Look at that, but it's only in one direction. 00:02:52.840 --> 00:02:54.080 Oh, yes! 00:02:54.640 --> 00:02:56.696 You don't see any spikes when I go forward, 00:02:56.720 --> 00:02:58.616 but they're all when I come back. NOTE Paragraph 00:02:58.640 --> 00:03:00.096 In our experiments, 00:03:00.120 --> 00:03:02.616 we were able to see that the neurons of the dragonfly 00:03:02.640 --> 00:03:05.896 fired when we moved the target in one direction but not the other. NOTE Paragraph 00:03:05.920 --> 00:03:07.176 Now, why is that? 00:03:07.200 --> 00:03:09.058 Remember when I said that the dragonfly 00:03:09.082 --> 00:03:11.136 had near 360-degree vision. 00:03:11.160 --> 00:03:13.656 Well, there's a section of the eye called the fovea 00:03:13.680 --> 00:03:16.576 and this is the part that has the sharpest visual acuity, 00:03:16.600 --> 00:03:19.296 and you can think of it as its crosshairs. 00:03:19.320 --> 00:03:23.496 Remember when I told you the dragonfly had individual precise control of its wings? 00:03:23.520 --> 00:03:27.576 When a dragonfly sees its prey, it trains its crosshairs on it 00:03:27.600 --> 00:03:30.976 and along its axons it sends messages only to the neurons 00:03:31.000 --> 00:03:32.696 that control the parts of the wings 00:03:32.720 --> 00:03:35.056 that are needed to keep that dragonfly on target. 00:03:35.080 --> 00:03:38.056 So if the prey is on the left of the dragonfly, 00:03:38.080 --> 00:03:41.856 only the neurons that are tugging the wings to the left are fired. 00:03:41.880 --> 00:03:44.376 And if the prey moves to the right of the dragonfly, 00:03:44.400 --> 00:03:47.576 those same neurons are not needed, so they're going to remain quiet. 00:03:47.600 --> 00:03:49.536 And the dragonfly speeds toward the prey 00:03:49.560 --> 00:03:52.816 at a fixed angle that's communicated by this crosshairs to the wings, 00:03:52.840 --> 00:03:54.856 and then boom, dinner. NOTE Paragraph 00:03:54.880 --> 00:03:58.576 Now, all this happens in a split second, and it's effortless for the dragonfly. 00:03:58.600 --> 00:04:00.176 It's almost like a reflex. 00:04:00.200 --> 00:04:04.296 And this whole incredibly efficient process is called fixation. NOTE Paragraph 00:04:04.320 --> 00:04:06.936 But there's one more story to this process. 00:04:06.960 --> 00:04:09.056 We saw how the neurons respond to movements, 00:04:09.080 --> 00:04:12.000 but how does the dragonfly know that something really is prey? 00:04:12.880 --> 00:04:14.216 This is where size matters. NOTE Paragraph 00:04:14.240 --> 00:04:16.760 Let's show the dragonfly a series of dots. 00:04:29.000 --> 00:04:30.216 Oh, yeah! NOTE Paragraph 00:04:30.240 --> 00:04:32.456 JS: Yeah, it prefers that one. NOTE Paragraph 00:04:32.480 --> 00:04:33.736 GG: Out of all the sizes, 00:04:33.760 --> 00:04:37.280 we found that the dragonfly responded to smaller targets over larger ones. 00:04:37.680 --> 00:04:41.736 In other words, the dragonfly was programmed to go after smaller flies 00:04:41.760 --> 00:04:44.096 versus something much larger, like a bird. 00:04:44.120 --> 00:04:46.696 And as soon as it recognizes something as prey, 00:04:46.720 --> 00:04:49.536 that poor little fly only has seconds to live. 00:04:49.560 --> 00:04:52.136 Today we got to see how the dragonfly's brain works 00:04:52.160 --> 00:04:53.840 to make it a very efficient killer. 00:04:54.440 --> 00:04:58.176 And let's be thankful that we didn't live 300 million years ago 00:04:58.200 --> 00:05:01.080 when dragonflies were the size of cats.