1 00:00:01,089 --> 00:00:05,634 Let’s take a look at the atoms of visual design: letterforms. 2 00:00:05,634 --> 00:00:09,695 This is the letter R, typeset in Gill Sans. 3 00:00:09,695 --> 00:00:13,877 Gill Sans was designed by Eric Gill and released in 1928. 4 00:00:13,877 --> 00:00:17,243 There’s a couple of things that I’d like to point out about this letter right here: 5 00:00:17,243 --> 00:00:23,023 One of them is that it has the perceptual characteristic of consistent weight all throughout; 6 00:00:23,023 --> 00:00:28,016 however, the actual thickness of the letter varies considerably over different parts. 7 00:00:28,016 --> 00:00:31,361 So, if you look at some points of the letter, like the bottom, 8 00:00:33,081 --> 00:00:38,000 you can see that they are much thicker than at other points in the letter. 9 00:00:41,028 --> 00:00:47,048 And what you see in these atoms is exactly what you’ll see throughout the entirety of graphic design, 10 00:00:47,048 --> 00:00:50,482 which is that we’re going for something that feels perceptually balanced — 11 00:00:50,482 --> 00:00:56,524 or may have perceptual uniformity — when the actuality may be quite different. 12 00:00:56,524 --> 00:01:03,067 This letter was also my very first assignment as a graphic design student at the Rhode Island School of Design: 13 00:01:03,067 --> 00:01:09,099 We had to draw this letter, one foot high, in black paint, on a white background. 14 00:01:09,099 --> 00:01:11,528 It may seem like a silly exercise, 15 00:01:11,528 --> 00:01:16,799 but it’s a great way to understand intimately the curves and strokes of the letterform, 16 00:01:16,799 --> 00:01:20,037 and it’s also really good at training both your eye and training your hand; 17 00:01:20,037 --> 00:01:25,058 Maybe it’s also a hazing ritual that I’ve just post-hoc rationalized. 18 00:01:25,058 --> 00:01:32,002 Anyhow, moving along, let’s look at the different parts of a typeface. 19 00:01:32,002 --> 00:01:38,006 One term you may be familiar with from using on your computer is the point size or the font size. 20 00:01:38,006 --> 00:01:40,059 And what’s notable about this is 21 00:01:40,059 --> 00:01:46,032 it seems like a number that ought to be set in stone and consistent across all typefaces, 22 00:01:46,032 --> 00:01:48,001 but it’s really not. 23 00:01:48,001 --> 00:01:53,001 Most of our terminology from typefaces comes from when they were set in lead, 24 00:01:53,001 --> 00:02:00,005 and the point size of a typeface really just means the height of the lead block that the type was set in, 25 00:02:00,005 --> 00:02:07,030 which has some relationship to the actual letter size, but is not completely consistent. 26 00:02:10,016 --> 00:02:12,621 For example, you can see, for the same point size, 27 00:02:12,621 --> 00:02:16,023 here’s a letter S in Gill Sans, and a letter S in Calibri, 28 00:02:16,023 --> 00:02:22,393 and you can that the Calibri S is lower than the Gill Sans S. 29 00:02:26,762 --> 00:02:29,001 Our second term is leading. 30 00:02:29,001 --> 00:02:32,524 If we have solid blocks of lead type that form the letters, 31 00:02:32,524 --> 00:02:39,016 the spacing that we get between the lines was achieved by putting thin strips of lead in between the lines, 32 00:02:39,016 --> 00:02:42,020 and that was called the leading. 33 00:02:42,020 --> 00:02:47,019 It’s customary to have 20% of your font size as your leading; 34 00:02:47,019 --> 00:02:51,015 and so, if in your word processor you’ve set 10-point type, 35 00:02:51,015 --> 00:02:59,036 it will usually automatically add an extra 2 points of spacing to put 12 points in between each line. 36 00:02:59,036 --> 00:03:05,045 The height of the lowercase letters is called the x-height, from the “x”. 37 00:03:05,045 --> 00:03:10,032 Some typefaces have a higher x-height, like Lucida Bright or Georgia; 38 00:03:10,032 --> 00:03:13,691 other typefaces have a lower x-height, like Baskerville. 39 00:03:13,691 --> 00:03:16,085 And this is important for design — especially online — 40 00:03:16,085 --> 00:03:23,074 because typefaces that have a higher x-height will be easier to read at smaller point sizes 41 00:03:23,074 --> 00:03:27,559 and on low resolution devices like most screens. 42 00:03:27,559 --> 00:03:31,079 Conversely, typefaces with a low x-height, like Baskerville, 43 00:03:31,079 --> 00:03:37,049 are used when you’re trying to give elegance, or a “Belle Époque” feel to something. 44 00:03:37,049 --> 00:03:39,781 And sprouting up and dropping down from the x-height 45 00:03:39,781 --> 00:03:44,960 are the ascenders — like the top stem of the h and the dot of the i —, 46 00:03:44,960 --> 00:03:49,560 and your descenders — like the y, p, g. 47 00:03:49,560 --> 00:03:55,214 In general, a typeface that has a low x-height will tend to have long ascenders and descenders, 48 00:03:55,214 --> 00:04:01,338 and a typeface that has a tall x-height will tend to have more squashed ascenders and descenders. 49 00:04:05,754 --> 00:04:08,079 Typefaces can also vary in their weight. 50 00:04:08,079 --> 00:04:15,459 So here have an example of light on the left, regular or roman in the middle, and bold on the right. 51 00:04:15,459 --> 00:04:19,087 Sometimes you’ll see weights in between like demibold or semibold, 52 00:04:19,087 --> 00:04:23,081 and, even to the further to the right of bold, you’ll see black. 53 00:04:23,081 --> 00:04:25,094 Number six on the list are serifs. 54 00:04:25,094 --> 00:04:33,070 Serifs are the doohickeys on the end of letters that give them that chiselled look. 55 00:04:33,070 --> 00:04:35,044 [There are] a couple more that are bonus: 56 00:04:35,044 --> 00:04:39,091 Some faces (typefaces) will also include small caps — which sure are exactly what they sound like, 57 00:04:39,091 --> 00:04:41,760 smaller versions of capital[s and] numbers. 58 00:04:41,760 --> 00:04:44,730 These can be used as headers, or they can also be used 59 00:04:44,730 --> 00:04:50,034 to put acronyms and other capital texts inside a block of body text. 60 00:04:51,592 --> 00:04:53,540 And one thing that you may not know is — 61 00:04:53,540 --> 00:04:59,677 just like letters come in uppercase and lowercase, numbers do too. 62 00:04:59,677 --> 00:05:01,094 These numbers, set in Neutraface, 63 00:05:01,094 --> 00:05:06,027 provide an example of something that has just small ascenders and descenders; 64 00:05:06,027 --> 00:05:09,462 in some cases these can be more dramatic. 65 00:05:09,462 --> 00:05:11,037 Lowercase numbers are useful 66 00:05:11,037 --> 00:05:17,001 when you want numbers to fit in with a larger block of text and not stand out. 67 00:05:17,001 --> 00:05:20,048 One of the most common questions I get asked about graphic design is: 68 00:05:20,048 --> 00:05:22,060 “Which typeface should I use?” 69 00:05:22,060 --> 00:05:24,063 And a lot of people have heard a story 70 00:05:24,063 --> 00:05:28,082 where serif typefaces are generally recommended for body text 71 00:05:28,082 --> 00:05:32,077 and sans serif typefaces are recommended for headers. 72 00:05:32,077 --> 00:05:36,956 And that’s, on the whole, not bad advice. 73 00:05:36,956 --> 00:05:41,644 There is an often-repeated claim that the reason that this is a good heuristic 74 00:05:41,644 --> 00:05:47,005 is because serif typefaces, with those doohickeys at the end, 75 00:05:47,005 --> 00:05:52,052 give your eyes something to latch onto, and consequently they’re easier to read. 76 00:05:52,052 --> 00:05:55,054 It’s an interesting hypothesis. 77 00:05:55,054 --> 00:05:58,007 So I looked into this a little bit for all of you, 78 00:05:58,007 --> 00:06:00,082 and there’s some challenges in trying to figure this out. 79 00:06:00,082 --> 00:06:07,088 For starters, individual differences in readers dwarf any manipulation effects of the typeface — 80 00:06:07,088 --> 00:06:09,711 like, some people read a whole lot faster than others, 81 00:06:09,711 --> 00:06:16,033 and so it’s difficult to measure differences in the readability of typefaces. 82 00:06:16,033 --> 00:06:19,081 Also, reading requires familiarity: 83 00:06:19,081 --> 00:06:23,076 It’s not like we’re born knowing what a letter R looks like; we need to practice. 84 00:06:23,076 --> 00:06:28,037 And so if you learn with one style of the letter R, you’ll probably be faster with that one; 85 00:06:28,037 --> 00:06:33,024 and if you learn with another style of the letter R, you’ll be faster with that one. 86 00:06:34,090 --> 00:06:37,523 If even when we got past all those challenges, 87 00:06:37,523 --> 00:06:41,026 it can be a little bit difficult to figure out exactly how you should measure this. 88 00:06:42,787 --> 00:06:48,018 Well, I can’t go so far as to say that the serif hypothesis is definitively untrue; 89 00:06:48,018 --> 00:06:53,026 what I can say is that there’s a lot of evidence to support that it actually is real. 90 00:06:53,026 --> 00:06:56,679 And Alex Poole does a great job of describing this on his web site: 91 00:06:57,760 --> 00:07:03,047 He writes that “What initially seemed a neat dichotomous question of serif versus sans serif 92 00:07:03,047 --> 00:07:07,071 has resulted in a body of research consisting of weak claims and counter-claims, 93 00:07:07,071 --> 00:07:10,653 and study after study with findings of ‘no difference’. 94 00:07:10,653 --> 00:07:16,050 Is it the case that more than one hundred years of research has been marred by [repeated] methodological flaws, 95 00:07:16,050 --> 00:07:21,041 or are serifs simply a typographical ‘red herring’?” 96 00:07:21,041 --> 00:07:26,035 It does seem that mixed-case type like you get in a sentence — upper and lower mixed together — 97 00:07:26,035 --> 00:07:29,773 is faster to read than all caps, presumably because 98 00:07:29,773 --> 00:07:34,257 you get greater vertical variation with the ascenders and descenders in lowercase letters, 99 00:07:34,257 --> 00:07:39,650 and that gives your eye more information as it’s reading. 100 00:07:39,650 --> 00:07:45,083 Also, in general, proportional fonts seem to be faster than monospaced fonts, like a typewriter. 101 00:07:45,083 --> 00:07:52,035 So, typewriter font[s] — slower to read — [but] that stuff only matters if you have to read a whole lot. 102 00:07:52,650 --> 00:07:59,623 But the simplest summary is that, in practice, legibility simply amounts to what you’re accustomed to. 103 00:08:00,162 --> 00:08:04,670 What do these words say? The bottom half is obscured. 104 00:08:04,670 --> 00:08:08,996 As many of you guessed, “I am a jelly donut”. 105 00:08:09,011 --> 00:08:13,472 How about this one here? Here the top half is obscured. 106 00:08:13,472 --> 00:08:15,408 Seems a little harder? 107 00:08:15,408 --> 00:08:19,027 “jfk spoke in berlin.” 108 00:08:19,027 --> 00:08:24,715 Turns out that more information seems to be encoded in the top half of texts than in the bottom half. 109 00:08:25,191 --> 00:08:27,450 And what does this say? 110 00:08:27,450 --> 00:08:31,611 [I] bet many of you saw it as “THE CAT”, 111 00:08:31,611 --> 00:08:36,039 and fewer people, “TAE CHT”. 112 00:08:36,039 --> 00:08:42,287 And that’s because expectation plays a really important role in what we read. 113 00:08:42,287 --> 00:08:44,029 And if you’re really perceptive, 114 00:08:44,029 --> 00:08:52,633 you may have realized that those two middle letters are actually exactly the same letter, 115 00:08:52,633 --> 00:08:57,051 so the only difference is what you’re expecting to see in each case. 116 00:08:57,051 --> 00:09:01,055 Your prior probability influences your interpretation. 117 00:09:01,055 --> 00:09:07,017 So, if you put all that together and return to our original question of “Which typeface should I use”, 118 00:09:07,017 --> 00:09:09,078 the answer is “It depends.” 119 00:09:09,078 --> 00:09:12,161 If you’re putting down a long block of text, 120 00:09:12,161 --> 00:09:16,351 it’s best to use a typeface that many people will be familiar with; 121 00:09:16,367 --> 00:09:21,455 and if you’re working online, use something that has a relatively high x-height. 122 00:09:21,455 --> 00:09:25,084 On the other hand, if you’re making a logo or a sign, 123 00:09:25,084 --> 00:09:29,046 or something that’s short and intended to be dramatic, 124 00:09:29,046 --> 00:09:33,001 then you probably want to use something a little bit funkier as your typeface — 125 00:09:33,001 --> 00:09:36,651 something that’s going to catch attention and be unique, memorable. 126 00:09:37,789 --> 00:09:42,625 And typefaces — like everything in life — [build] up reputation. 127 00:09:42,625 --> 00:09:46,694 So, over time, certain typefaces come to mean certain things. 128 00:09:46,694 --> 00:09:50,910 And here’s a great example from Hoefler and Frere-Jones, writing on their blog. 129 00:09:50,910 --> 00:09:56,033 They talk about how Baskerville seems like it’s out of a literary magazine 130 00:09:56,033 --> 00:10:03,071 and Optima seems to remind us of cheap 1950’s hygiene products. 131 00:10:03,071 --> 00:10:08,009 Here are two easy exercises you can use to improve your typographical eye: 132 00:10:08,009 --> 00:10:14,046 The first one is: Look around you for all of the different ways that the same typeface gets deployed; 133 00:10:14,046 --> 00:10:17,044 that will give you a sense of its range. 134 00:10:17,044 --> 00:10:25,619 And the second one is: Look at how the same text, with different typefaces, really changes its meaning. 135 00:10:27,157 --> 00:10:30,033 In this class we have a lot of ground to cover quickly, 136 00:10:30,033 --> 00:10:33,071 and so unfortunately we can’t spend too much time on typography; 137 00:10:33,071 --> 00:10:36,056 but it’s a fascinating subject, and if you’d like to learn more about this, 138 00:10:36,056 --> 00:10:42,035 there are many great books and design resources online, and they’re growing rapidly. 139 00:10:42,035 --> 00:10:48,005 Here are just a few that I think are particularly good.