(Lia Commissar) Hi! This morning I'm going to talk about education and neuroscience, both generally as a field that's developing but also, the specific work that we've been doing in this field. The Wellcome Trust -- OK -- the Wellcome Trust, for those of you who don't know, is the second largest charitable foundation globally, with the aim of improving health. And it does that by funding lots of bio-medical research, but also by funding work in the social science and humanities, funding lots of education work, doing lots of engagement work and also lots of policy work. So I'm going to talk through a few things this morning. Firstly, what has neuroscience got to do with education? What impact does it have on education at the moment? The work that we've been doing and then, thinking about the future. So, what has neuroscience got to do with education? Well, if education is about learning something, be it knowledge or a skill, and neuroscience is the study of the nervous system and the brain, then they're pretty linked, in my opinion. And it's not new to kind of talk about the brain, in relation to education. People have been discussing this, critiquing it, debating it for lots of years. There is loads of research papers out there with titles such as "A Bridge too far", "Its a Prime time to build a bridge", "A two-way path is possible", "Booting the bridge from by thence". (check) So lots of people have been talking about this bridge but not many people have been building it. And, this is kind of understandable because its probably only really in the last 10 years that neuroscience is, the understanding from neuroscience has evolved significantly that we can start to think about how it may have implications for the classroom. And this is really exciting and there is lots of potential and its really understanding, you can understand why teachers, why policy makers, why technologists want to start applying some of these ideas to education. But my word of warning that will go throughout this presentation really is about waiting and doing the research and finding out whether these things are actually going to be helpful and impactful in the classroom. And the other thing that I must say just before i go on is that I am not saying in any way that neuroscience alone is the answer and its going to solve all our problems or can tell us everything about the classroom,: that's a very complex ecosystem. But, neuroscience is a kind of a new-ish field where we can start using that alongside psychology, alongside educational research and alongside teacher's knowledge to build something really good for the future and see if we can start improving learning in the class room. 2.51 So, what impact does neuroscience have on education? So, we're at an education conference -- I thought I'd wait and see what you guys think. So, I'm going to put a few statements up on the board, as any good teacher does. And what I would like you to do is just simply raise your hand if you agree with the statement, if you think it's true. So: "We mostly only use 10% of our brain." Hands up if you agree. OK. I will just show you this: These are some stats, I'll show you the paper this comes from in a second. This was a survey of teachers in five countries and those are the percentages of the teachers that agree. Hum -- it's not really working, but I can tell you that this is not true -- uh, there we go -- even sat there now, you might think you're not doing very much but your brain is still in control of your breathing, in control of your heart rate, keeping you standing up, sitting upright, perhaps paying attention, maybe not, maybe mind-wandering but it's all using your brain, all parts of your brain. So that's one of these myths. Second one: "Individuals lean better when they receive information "in their preferred learning style." So visually, by seeing, auditory, by hearing, or kinesthetically, by doing things. Hands up if you agree with that statement. OK. You can see, across the board, teachers in lots of countries agree with this. (Laughter) Yes. So (she laughs) It is true that people have a preference. You might prefer to learn something in a particular way, you might feel that you are a visual learner, and that you learn better in that, by using stuff visually: you do have a preference; but researches show that you don't learn any better if you are showing the material in that particular way, believe it or not. One last one: "Differences in hemispheric dominance, left or right brain, can help to explain "individual differences amongst learners." Hands up if you agree. We might start to see a pattern, I don't know. (Laughter) So, here is some stats. Really high in the UK. Again, it's not true. If you hear things about integrating the right or left brain, or if you hear things about one side of your brain is the logical side and the other side is more mathsy -- sorry, creative, it's just not true. And I'm going to skip the next one. ["Regular drinking of caffeinated drinks reduces alertness."] But this one is true. (Laughter) OK, I can tell you a little more about that. If you want to see more about these neuromyths, This is a really good paper written by Paul Howard Jones last year. It got a lot of interest on social media and it goes through lots of what these common neuromyths are. So, neuromyths: Where do they come from? Are they a problem? And what to do about them? So, generally they often come from some kind of science that's been over- or misinterpreted, and they tend to stick around (excuse me) because they are easy to understand or easy to kind of implement in the classroom. Are they a problem? Well, if you think you only use 10% of your brain, is that a problem? Possibly not, it's not accurate, but when I first started teaching about nine years ago, and this was common in lots of schools across the UK, I'm not sure about in other countries, students were asked to fill a questionnaire about how they learned, and did they like learning in this way. And teachers were given a spreadsheet with students and which were their preferred learning styles. And teachers were encouraged to make sure that they were providing information for those students in that particular way. And I'd say that is a bit of a problem, because teachers were potentially wasting time or feeling that they should be doing something that was not actually benefiting the students. A positive outcome was that lessons became more diverse, ............... (check) about a range of activities but at the same time, students would say things like: "Miss, I'm not doing this activity because I'm a kinesthetic learner." And so students were themselves limiting themselves and not getting the benefit of learning in multiple modalities, which is the way you learn best, by using different ways of learning the same information. 7:19 So, what to do about them? Well, I might come on to that a bit later in what we're going to do. So, the research is carrying on, but lots of research in this area is still at the stage of it's about cells in a Petri dish It might be about finding out about what's going on in animals, or very small-scale trials of humans, but maybe in a lab. Not much is going on in the classroom. And so we set p this initiative, Education Neuroscience Initiative with the Education Endowment Foundation, who are the What Works Centre of Education in the UK. And we did it for two reasons: we wanted to build the evidence of what works in education, informed by neuroscience, and we also wanted to help support teachers and general educators with what we do and don't know. (check) So, we firstly launched a funding round, to get people to apply for money to do research in the space, and all the projects had to have some evidence that they will -- that they work, some pilot data. But they also had to be scalable and affordable, so that if we find that these things worked, that they could be rolled out, that they could lead to policy changes, that they could be available to everybody. And there is particular emphasis as well around disadvantaged students. We want to do something that would benefit all, and try and close the socioeconomic gap. And all of our projects are also paired with an independent evaluator. So, you have your project team and you also have an evaluation team. And that evaluation team helps to shape the methodology that's used, make sure it's robust, educationally. They also collect some of the first data and they also report on that data first. So there is no chance for the project team to maybe overstate claims about what they found. So we funded these projects and these are all taking place in around 50 to 100 schools, each in the UK. I won't talk about all of them, but I'll just whiz through a couple. Fit to Study is looking at great research around how more vigorous activity can influence learning, short- and long-term. We know this from lots of research in animals and from small-group studies, but nobody has done anything in the UK on this scale. And so we're looking at changing what happens in P.E. lessons, and then measuring the short- and long-term outcomes of those students. Teen Sleep has had a certain media coverage. And that's looking at we know, researchers know, neuroscientists will tell you, we know that teens have a different sleep-wake cycle, their circadian rhythm is altered by a couple of hours. So they don't feel tired in the evenings, it's really hard to get them up in the morning, they are not fit and ready to start learning early in the morning. So we're studying, at a late school start time against a sleep education program, where we just teach them about the importance of their sleep, how to get good sleep, about using technology just before bed and about how that might affect their sleep -- not in a positive way -- and looking again at their academic outcomes. 10:25