Back in 2003,
the UK government carried out a survey,
and it was a survey that measured
levels of numeracy in the population,
and they were shocked to find out
that for every 100 working aged adults,
in the country, 47 of them
lacked level one numeracy skills.
Now, level one numeracy skills,
that's low-end GCSE score.
It's the ability to deal with fractions,
percentages, and decimals.
So this figure prompted
a lot of handwringing in Whitehall.
Policies were changed,
investments were made,
and then they ran
the survey again in 2011.
So can you guess what
happened to this number?
It went up to 49.
(Laughter)
And in fact, when I reported
this figure in the FT,
one of our readers joked and said,
"This figure is only shocking
to 51 percent of the population."
(Laughter)
But I preferred, actually,
the reaction of a schoolchild
who said to me when I presented
at a school this information,
raised their hand and said,
"How do we know that the person
who made that number
isn't one of the 49 percent either?"
(Laughter)
So clearly, there's a numeracy issue,
because these are important
skills for life, and a lot of the changes
that we want to introduce in this century
involve us becoming
more comfortable with numbers.
Now, it's not just an English problem.
OECD this year released some figures
looking at numeracy in young people,
and leading the way, the USA,
nearly 40 percent of young people
in the US have low numeracy.
Now, England is there too,
but there are seven countries,
seven OECD countries
with figures above 20 percent,
and that is a problem,
because it doesn't have to be that way.
If you look at the far end of this graph,
you can see the Netherlands and Korea,
they're in single figures.
Okay? So there's definitely a numeracy
problem that we want to address.
Now, as useful as studies like these are,
I think we risk herding people
inadvertently into one of two categories,
that there are two kinds of people:
those people that are comfortable
with numbers, that can do numbers,
and the people who can't.
And I suppose what I'm trying
to talk about here today is to say
that I believe that that
is a false dichotomy.
It's not an immutable pairing.
I think you don't have to have
tremendously high levels of numeracy
to be inspired by numbers,
and that that should be the starting point
to the journey ahead.
And one of the ways in which
we can begin that journey, for me,
is looking at statistics.
Now, I am the first to acknowledge
that statistics has got somewhat
of an image problem.
It's the part of mathematics
that even mathematicians
don't particularly like,
because whereas the rest of math
is all about precision and certainty,
statistics is almost the reverse of that.
But actually, you know, I was a late
convert to the world of statistics myself.
If you'd asked my undergraduate professors
what two subjects would I be
least likely to excel in after university,
they would have told you statistics
and computer programming,
and yet here I am about to show you some
statistical graphics that I programmed.
So what inspired that change in me?
What made me think that statistics
was actually an interesting thing?
It's really because
statistics are about us.
If you look at the etymology
of the word statistics,
it's the science of dealing with data
about the state or the community
that we live in.
So statistics are about us as a group,
not us as individuals,
and I think as social animals,
we share this fascination about how
we as individuals relate to our groups,
to our peers.
And statistics in this way
are at their most powerful
when they surprise us.
And there's been some really wonderful
surveys carried out recently
by Ipsos MORI the last few years.
They did a survey of over
a thousand adults in the UK,
and said, okay, for every 100 people
in England and Wales,
how many of them are Muslim?
Now the average answer from this survey,
which was supposed to be representative
of the total population, was 24.
That's what people thought.
British people think 24 out of every 100
people in the country are Muslim.
Now, official figures reveal
that figure to be about five.
So there's this big variation between
what we think, our perception,
and the reality as given by statistics.
And I think that's interesting.
What could possibly be causing
that misperception?
And I was so thrilled with this study,
I started to take questions out
in presentations. I was referring to it.
Now, I did a presentation at St. Paul's
School for Girls in Hammersmith,
and I had an audience rather like this,
except it was comprised entirely
of sixth form girls,
and I said, "Girls,
how many teenage girls do you think
the British public think
get pregnant every year?"
And the girls were apoplectic when I said
the British public think that 15
out of every 100 teenage girls
get pregnant in the year.
And they had every right to be angry,
because in fact, I'd have to have closer
to 200 dots before I could color one in,
in terms of what
the official figures tell us.
And rather like numeracy,
this is not just an English problem.
Ipsos MORI expanded this survey
in recent years to go across the world.
And so, they asked Saudi Arabians,
for every 100 adults in your country,
how many of them are overweight or obese?
And the average answer from the Saudis
was just over a quarter.
That's what they thought.
Just over a quarter of adults
are overweight or obese.
The official figures show, actually,
it's nearer to three quarters.
(Laughter)
So again, a big variation.
And I love this one: they asked in Japan,
they asked the Japanese,
for every 100 Japanese people,
how many of them live in rural areas?
And the average, again,
this is the average,
was about a 50-50 split,
just over halfway.
They thought 56 out of every 100
Japanese people lived in rural areas.
The official figure is seven.
So extraordinary variations,
and surprising to some,
but not surprising to people
who have read the work
of Daniel Kahneman, for example,
the Nobel-winning economist.
Him and his colleague, Amos Tversky
spent years researching this disjoint
between what people perceive
and the reality,
the fact that people are actually
pretty poor intuitive statisticians.
And there are many reasons for this.
Individual experiences certainly
can influence our perceptions,
but so too can things like the media
reporting things by exception
rather than what's normal.
Kahneman had a nice way
of referring to that.
He said, "We can be blind
to the obvious" --
so we've got the numbers wrong --
"but we can be blind
to our blindness about it."
And that has enormous
repercussions for decision making.
So at the statistics office
while this was all going on,
I thought this was really interesting.
I said, you know,
this is clearly a global problem,
but maybe geography is the issue here.
These were questions that were all about
how well do you know your country?
So in this case, it's how well
do you know 64 million people?
Not very well, it turns out.
I can't do that.
So I had an idea, which was
to think about this same sort of approach
but to think about it
in a very local sense.
Is this a local?
If we reframed the questions and say,
how well do you know your local area,
would your answers be any more accurate?
So I devised a quiz:
how well do you know your area?
It's a simple web app.
You put in a post code,
and then it will ask you questions
based on census data for your local area.
And I was very conscious
in designing this.
I wanted to make it open
to the widest possible range of people,
not just the 49 percent
who can get the numbers.
I wanted everyone to engage with it.
So for the design of the quiz,
I was inspired by the isotypes
of Otto Neurath from the 1920s and '30s.
Now, these are methods for representing
numbers using repeating icons,
and the numbers are there,
but they sit in the background.
So it's a great way of representing
quantity without resorting to using
terms like percentage,
fractions, and ratios.
So here's the quiz.
The layout of the quiz is,
you have your repeating icons
on the lefthand side there,
and a map showing you the area
we're asking you questions about
on the righthand side.
There are seven questions.
Each question, there's a possible answer
between zero and a hundred,
and at the end of the quiz,
you get an overall score
between zero and a hundred.
And so because this is TEDxExeter,
I thought we would have a quick look
at the quiz for the first few questions
of Exeter.
And so the first question is,
for every 100 people,
how many are aged under 16?
Now, I don't know Exeter very well at all,
so I had a guess at this,
but it gives you an idea
of how this quiz works.
You drag the slider
to highlight your icons,
and then just click submit to answer,
and we animate away the difference
between your answer and reality.
And it turns out I was
a pretty terrible guess.
Five.
How about the next question?
This is asking about
what the average age is,
so the age at which half
the population are younger
and half the population are older.
And I thought 35: that sounds
middle-aged to me.
Actually in Exeter it's incredibly young,
and I had underestimated the impact
of the university in this area.
The questions get harder
as you go through.
So this one's now asking
about homeownership.
For every 100 households, how many
are owned with a mortgage or loan?
And I hedged my bets here,
because I didn't want to be
more than 50 out on the answer.
(Laughter)
And actually, these get harder,
these questions,
because when you're in an area,
when you're in a community,
things like age, it's pretty,
there are clues
to whether a population is old or young.
Just by looking around
the area, you can see it.
Something like homeownership
is much more difficult to see,
so we revert to our own heuristics,
our own biases about how many
people we think own their own homes.
Now, the truth is,
when we published this quiz,
the census data that it's based on
was already a few years old.
We've had online applications
that allow you to put in a post code
and get statistics back for years.
So in some senses,
this was all a little bit old
and not necessarily new,
but I was interested to see
what reaction we might get
by game-ifying the data
in the way that we have,
by using animation
and playing on the fact that
people have their own preconceptions.
It turns out, the reaction was
more than I could have hoped for.
It was a long-held ambition of mine
to bring down a statistics website
due to public demand.
(Laughter)
This url complains the word
statistics, gov, and UK,
which is three of people's
least favorite words in a url,
and the amazing thing about this
was that the website came down
at quarter to 10 at night
because people were actually
engaging with this data
of their own free will,
using their own personal time.
I was very interested to see that we got
something like a quarter of a million
people playing the quiz within
the space of 48 hours of launching it,
and it sparked an enormous discussion
online, on social media,
which was largely dominated
by people having fun
with their misconceptions,
which is something that I couldn't have
hoped for any better, in some respects.
I also liked the fact that people started
sending it to politicians.
How well do you know the area
you claim to represent?
(Laughter)
And then just to finish,
going back to the two kinds of people,
I thought it would be really interesting
to see how people who are good
with numbers would do in this quiz.
The national statistician
of England and Wales, John Pullinger,
you would expect he would be pretty good.
He got 44 for his own area.
Jeremy Paxman, admittedly
after a glass of wine, 36.
Even worse. Okay?
It just shows you that the numbers
can inspire us all.
They can surprise us all.
So very often, we talk about statistics
as being the science of uncertainty.
My parting thought today
is actually statistics
is the science of us,
and that's why we should
be fascinated by numbers.
Thank you very much.
(Applause)