So I'd like to talk about
the development of human potential,
and I'd like to start with maybe the most
impactful modern story of development.
Many of you here have probably heard
of the 10,000 hours rule.
Maybe you even model
your own life after it.
Basically it's the idea
that to become great in anything,
it takes 10,000 hours
of focused practice,
so you'd better get started
as early as possible.
The poster child
for this story is Tiger Woods.
His father famously gave him
a putter when he was seven months old.
At 10 months, he started
imitating his father's swing.
At two, you can go on YouTube
and see him on national television.
Fast-forward to the age of 21,
he's the greatest golfer in the world.
Quintessential 10,000 hours story.
Another that features a number
of bestselling books
is that of the three Polgar sisters,
whose father decided to teach
them chess in a very technical manner
from a very early age.
And really he wanted to show
that with a head start
in focused practice,
any child could become
a genius in anything.
And in fact,
two of his daughters went on
to become grandmaster chess players.
So when I became the science writer
at "Sports Illustrated" magazine,
I got curious.
If this 10,000 hours rule is correct,
then we should see that elite
athletes get a head start
in so-called "deliberate practice."
This is coached,
error-correction focused practice,
not just playing around.
And in fact,
when scientists study elite athletes,
they see that they spend
more time in deliberate practice ...
not a big surprise.
When they actually track athletes
over the course of their development,
the pattern looks like this:
the future elites actually spend
less time early on in deliberate practice
in their eventual sport.
They tend to have what scientists
call a "sampling period,"
where they try a variety
of physical activities,
they gain broad, general skills,
they learn about
their interests and abilities
and delay specializing until later
than peers who plateau at lower levels.
And so when I saw that I said,
"Gosh, that doesn't really comport
with the 10,000 hours rule, does it?"
So I started to wonder about other domains
that we associate with obligatory,
early specialization,
like music.
Turns out the pattern's often similar.
This is research from
a world-class music academy,
and what I want to draw
your attention to is this:
the exceptional musicians didn't start
spending more time in deliberate practice
than the average musicians
until their third instrument.
They, too, tended to have
a sampling period.
Even musicians we think of
as famously precocious,
like Yo-Yo Ma.
He had a sampling period,
he just went through it more rapidly
than most musicians do.
Nonetheless, this research
is almost entirely ignored,
and much more impactful
is the first page of the book
"Battle Hymn of the Tiger Mother,"
where the author recounts
assigning her daughter violin.
Nobody seems to remember
the part later in the book
where her daughter turns to her
and says, "You picked it, not me,"
and largely quits.
So having seen this sort of surprising
pattern in sports and music,
I started to wonder about domains
that affect even more people,
like education.
An economist found a natural experiment
in the higher-ed systems
of England and Scotland.
In the period he studied
the systems were very similar
except in England,
students had to specialize
in their mid-teen years
to pick a specific course
of study to apply to,
whereas in Scotland,
they could keep trying things
in the university if they wanted to.
And his question was:
who wins the trade-off,
the early of the late specializers?
And what he saw was that the early
specializers jump out to an income lead
because they have more
domain-specific skills.
The late specializers get to try
more different things,
and when they do pick,
they have better fit,
or what economists call "match quality."
And so their growth rates are faster.
By six years out,
they erase that income gap.
Meanwhile, the early specializers
start quitting their career tracks
in much higher numbers,
essentially because they were
made to choose so early
that they more often made poor choices.
So the late specializers lose
in the short term
and win the long run.
I think if we thought about
career choice like dating,
we might not pressure people
to settle down quite so quickly.
So this got me interested --
seeing this pattern again --
in exploring the developmental backgrounds
of people whose work I had long admired.
Like Duke Ellington,
who shunned music lessons
as a kid to focus on baseball
and painting and drawing.
Or Maryam Mirzakhani,
who wasn't interested
in math as a girl --
dreamed of becoming a novelist --
and went on to become the first
and so far only woman
to win the Fields Medal,
the most prestigious prize
in the world in math.
Or Vincent Van Gogh --
had five different careers,
each of which he deemed
his true calling
before flaming out spectacularly.
And in his late 20s picked up a book
called "The Guide to the ABCs of Drawing."
That worked out OK.
Claude Shannon was an electrical engineer
at the University of Michigan
who took a philosophy course
just to fulfill a requirement,
and in it, he learned about
a near century-old system of logic
by which true and false statements
could be coded as ones and zeros
and solved like math problems.
This led to the development
of binary code,
which underlies all of our
digital computers today.
Finally, my own sort of role model,
Frances Hesselbein --
this is me with her --
she took her first professional
job at the age of 54
and went on to become
the CEO of the Girl Scouts,
which she saved.
She tripled minority membership,
added 130,000 volunteers,
and this is one of the proficiency badges
that came out of her tenure --
it's binary code for girls
learning about computers.
Today, Frances runs a leadership institute
where she works every
weekday in Manhattan,
and she's only 104,
so who knows what's next.
(Laughter)
We never really hear developmental
stories like this, do we?
We don't hear about the research
that found that Nobel laureate scientists
are 22 times more likely
to have a hobby outside of work
as are typical scientists.
We never hear that.
Even when the performers
or the work is very famous,
we don't hear these
developmental stories.
For example, here's
an athlete I've followed.
Here he is at age six,
wearing a Scottish rugby kit.
He tried some tennis,
some skiing,
wrestling.
His mother was actually a tennis coach
but she declined to coach him
because he wouldn't return balls normally.
He did some basketball,
table tennis, swimming.
When his coaches wanted to move
him up a level to play with older boys,
he declined because he just wanted
to talk about pro wrestling
after practice with his friends.
And he kept trying more sports:
handball, volleyball, soccer,
badminton, skateboarding --
so who is this dabbler?
This is Roger Federer.
Every bit as famous
as an adult as Tiger Woods,
and yet even tennis enthusiasts
don't usually know anything
about his developmental story.
Why is that even though it's the norm?
I think it's partly because the Tiger
story is very dramatic.
But also because it seems
like this tidy narrative
that we can extrapolate to anything
that we want to be good
at in our own lives.
But that I think is a problem,
because it turns out that in many ways
golf is a uniquely horrible model
of almost everything
that humans want to learn.
(Laughter)
Golf is the epitome of what
the psychologist Robin Hogarth called
a "kind" learning environment.
Kind learning environments
have next steps and goals that are clear,
rules that are clear and never change --
when you do something you get feedback
that is quick and accurate;
work next year will look
like work last year.
Chess:
also a kind learning environment.
The grandmasters' advantage is based
on knowledge of recurring patterns,
which is also why
it's so easy to automate.
On the other end of the spectrum
are "wicked" learning environments,
where next steps and goals
may not be clear.
Rules may change.
You may or may not get feedback
when you do something.
It may be delayed,
it may be inaccurate
and work next year may not
look like work last year.
So which one of these
sounds like the world
we're increasingly living in?
In fact, our need to think
in an adaptable manner
and to keep track of interconnecting parts
has fundamentally changed our perception
so that when you look at this diagram,
the central circle on the right
probably looks larger to you
because your brain is drawn
to the relationship of the parts
in the whole,
whereas someone who hasn't been
exposed to modern work
with its requirement for adaptable,
conceptual thought,
will see correctly that the central
circles are the same size.
So here we are in the wicked work world,
and there, sometimes
hyperspecialization can backfire badly.
For example,
in research in a dozen countries
that matched people
for their parent's years of education,
their test scores,
their own years of education,
the difference was some got
career-focused education
and some got broader, general education.
The pattern was those who got
the career-focused education
are more likely to be hired
right out of training,
more likely to make more money right away,
but so much less adaptable
in a changing work world
that they spend so much less time
in the workforce overall
that they win in the short term
and lose in the long run.
Or consider a famous,
20-year study of experts
making geopolitical
and economic predictions.
The worst forecasters
were the most specialized experts.
Those who'd spent their entire careers
studying one or two problems
and came to see the whole world
through one lens or mental model.
Some of them actually got worse
as they accumulated
experience and credentials.
The best forecasters were simply
bright people with wide-ranging interests.
Now in some domains,
like medicine,
increasing specialization has been
both inevitable and beneficial.
No question about it.
And yet it's been a double-edged sword.
A few years ago,
one of the most popular surgeries
in the world for knee pain
was tested in a placebo-controlled trial.
Some of the patients got sham surgery.
That means the surgeons make an incision,
they bang around like
they're doing something,
then they sew the patient back up.
That performed just as a well.
And yet surgeons who specialize
in the procedure continue to do it
by the millions.
So if hyperspecialization isn't always
the trick in a wicked world, what is?
That can be difficult to talk about
because it doesn't
always look like this path.
Sometimes it looks like
meandering or zigzagging
or keeping a broader view.
It can look like getting behind.
But I want to talk about what
some of those tricks might be.
If we look at research
on technological innovation,
it shows that increasingly,
the most impactful patents
are not authored by individuals
who drill deeper, deeper, deeper
into one area of technology,
as classified by the US Patent Office,
but rather by teams
that include individuals
who have worked across a large number
of different technology classes
and often merge things
from different domains.
Someone whose work I've admired
who was sort of
on the forefront of this --
a Japanese man named Gunpei Yokoi.
Yokoi didn't score well
on his electronics exams at school,
so he had to settle for a low-tier job
as a machine maintenance worker
at a playing card company in Kyoto.
He realized he wasn't equipped
to work on the cutting edge,
but that there was so much
information easily available
that maybe he could combine things
that were already well-known
in ways that specialists
were too narrow to see.
So he combined some well-known technology
from the calculator industry
with some well-known technology
from the credit card industry
and made handheld games.
And they were a hit.
And it turned this playing card company,
which was founded in a wooden
storefront in the 19th century,
into a toy and game operation.
You may have heard of it;
It's called Nintendo.
Yokoi's creative philosophy translated
to "lateral thinking
with withered technology;"
taking well-known technology
and using it in new ways.
And his magnum opus was this:
the Game Boy.
Technological joke in every way.
And it came out at the same time
as color competitors from Saga and Atari
and it blew them away
because Yokoi knew what his
customers cared about wasn't color.
It was durability,
portability,
affordability,
battery life --
game selection.
This is mine that I found
in my parents' basement --
(Laughter)
seen better days.
But you can see the red light is on.
I flipped it on and played some Tetris,
which I thought was especially impressive
because the batteries had expired
in 2007 and 2013.
(Laughter)
So this breadth advantage holds
in more subjective realms as well.
In a fascinating study of what leads
some comic book creators
to be more likely to make
blockbuster comics,
a pair of researchers found
that it was neither the number of years
of experience in the field
nor the resources of the publisher,
nor the number of previous comics made.
It was the number of different genres
that a creator had worked across.
And interestingly, a broad individual
could not be entirely replaced
by a team of specialists.
We probably don't make as many
of those people as we could
because early on,
they just look like they're behind
and we don't tend to incentivize anything
that doesn't look like a head start
or specialization.
In fact, I think in the well-meaning
drive for a head start,
we often even counterproductively
short circuit even the way we learn
new material at a fundamental level.
In a study last year,
seventh-grade math classrooms in the US
were randomly assigned
to different types of learning.
Some got what's called blocked practice.
That's like, you get problem type A,
AAAAA,
BBBBB,
and so on.
Progress is fast,
kids are happy,
everything's great.
Other classrooms got assigned
to what's called "Interleaved practice."
That's like if you took
all the problem types
and put them in a hat
and drew them out at random.
Progress is slower,
kids are more frustrated.
But instead of learning
how to execute procedures,
they're learning how to match
a strategy to a type of problem.
And when the test comes around,
the interleaved group blew
the block practice group away.
It wasn't even close.
Now I found a lot of this research
deeply counterintuitive.
They idea that a headstart,
whether in picking a career
or a course of study
of just in learning new material,
can sometimes undermine
long-term development.
And naturally, I think there are as many
ways to succeed as there are people,
but I think we tend only to incentivize
and encourage the Tiger path
when increasingly in a wicked world,
we need people who travel
the Roger path as well.
Or as the eminent physicist
and mathematician
and wonderful writer,
Freeman Dyson put it --
and Dyson passed away yesterday,
so I hope I'm doing
his words honor here --
as he said,
"For a healthy ecosystem,
we need both birds and frogs.
Frogs are down in the mud,
see all the granular details.
The birds are soaring up above
not seeing those details,
but integrating
the knowledge of the frogs."
And we need both.
The problem, Dyson said,
is that "we're telling everyone
to become frogs"
and I think,
in a wicked world
that's increasingly short-sighted.
Thank you very much.
(Applause)