Can week-old pizza cause
psychedelic hallucinations?
Does coffee makes you smarter
or does it just make you
do dumb stuff faster?
Like much much of psychology itself,
questions like this can seem pretty intuitive.
I mean, people may not be the easiest
organisms to understand, but...
You're a person, right?
So you must be qualified to draw, like,
some conclusions about other people,
and what makes them tick.
But it's important to realize that
your intuition isn't always right.
In fact, sometimes it is exactly wrong,
and we tend to grossly underestimate
the dangers of false intuition.
If you have some idea about a person and
their behavior that turns out to be right,
that reinforces your trust in your intuition.
Like if I warned my buddy Bob
against eating the deep-dish pizza
that's been in the fridge for the past week,
but he eats it anyway and
soon starts to wig out,
I'm gonna say: "Dude, I told you so!"
But if I'm wrong, and he's totally fine,
I'll probably won't even
think about it ever again.
This is known as hindsight bias,
or the "I-knew-it-all-along" phenomenon.
This doesn't mean that common sense is wrong,
it just mean that our intuitive sense
more easily describes what JUST happened
than what WILL happen in the future.
Another reason you can't
blindly trust your intuition
is your natural tendency toward overconfidence.
Sometimes, you just really, really
feel like you're right about people
when actually, you're really, really wrong!
We've all been there...
We also tend to perceive order in random events,
which can lead to false assumptions.
For example, if you flip a coin five times,
you have equal chances of getting all tails
as you do getting alternating heads and tails,
but we see the series of five tails
as something unusual, as a streak,
and thus, giving that result some kind of
meaning that it very definitely does not have.
That is why we have the methods and safeguards
of psychological research and experimentation,
and the glorious process of scientific inquiry.
They help us to get around these problems
and basically, save the study of our
minds from the stupidity of our minds.
So I hope that it won't be a spoiler if I
tell you now that pizza won't make you trip,
and coffee doesn't make you smart. Sorry.
[on-screen animations and
ribbons of science sentences]
Title screen says "Episode 2:
Research & Experimentation."
In most ways, psychological research
is no different than in the
other scientific discipline.
Like, step one is always figuring out how to
ask general questions about your subject,
and turn them into measurable,
testable propositions.
This is called "operationalizing" your questions.
So you know how the scientific method works.
It starts with a question and a theory.
And I don't mean theory in the
sense of like, a hunch that says
"a quad-shot of espresso
makes you think better."
Instead, in science, a theory is what explains
and organizes lots of different observations
and predicts outcomes.
And when you come up
with a testable prediction,
that's your hypothesis.
Once your theory and hypothesis are in place
you need a clear and common
language to report them with.
So, for example,
defining exactly what you mean
by "thinking better" with
your espresso hypothesis
would allow other researchers
to replicate the experiment.
And replication is key.
You can watch a person exhibit
a certain behavior once,
and it won't prove very much.
But if you keep getting consistent results
even as you change subjects or situations,
you're probably onto something.
This is a problem with one popular
type of psychological research:
case studies, which take an
in-depth look at one individual.
Case studies can sometimes be misleading,
because by their nature,
they can't be replicated;
so, they run the risk of over-generalizing.
Still, they're good at showing
us what CAN happen,
and end up framing questions for more
extensive and generalizable studies.
They're also often memorable
and a great story-telling device psychologists
use to observe and describe behavior.
Like, say, the smell of coffee makes
Carl suddenly anxious and irritable.
That obviously doesn't mean that it
has the same effect on everyone.
In fact, Carl has terrible memories
associated with that smell,
and so his case is actually quite rare.
Poor Carl... :(
But, you will still have to look at lots of
other cases to determine that conclusively.
Another popular method of psychological
research is naturalistic observation,
where researchers simply watch
behavior in a natural environment,
whether that's chimps poking
anthills in the jungle,
kids clowning in a classroom,
or drunk dudes yelling at soccer games.
The idea is to let the subjects
just "do their thing"
without trying to manipulate
or control the situation.
So yeah, basically just spying on people.
Like case studies, naturalistic observations
are great at describing behavior,
but they're very limited in explaining it.
Psychologists can also collect behavioral
data using surveys or interviews,
asking people to report
their opinions and behaviors.
Sexuality researcher Alfred Kinsey
famously used this technique
when he surveyed thousands of men
and women on their sexual history
and published his findings.
in a pair of revolutionary texts:
"Sexual Behavior in the Human Male"
and "Sexual Behavior in the Human Female."
Surveys are a great way to access people's
consciously held attitudes and beliefs,
but how to ask the questions can be tricky;
subtle word choices can influence results.
For example, more forceful
words like "ban" or "censor"
may elicit different reactions
than "limit" or "not allow."
Asking: "Do you believe in space aliens?"
is a much different question than
"Do you think that there is intelligent
life somewhere else in the universe?"
It's the same question, but in the first,
the subject may assume that you mean
"aliens visiting the Earth
and making crop circles
and abducting people and poking them."
And if how you phrase surveys is important,
so is who you ask.
I could ask a room full of students at a pacifist
club what they think about arms control,
but the results wouldn't be a representative
measure of where the students stand,
because there's a pretty clear
sampling bias at work here.
To fairly represent a population,
I'd need to give a random sample
where all members of the target group
(in this case, students) had an equal chance
of being selected to answer the question.
So, once you've described behavior
with surveys, case studies,
or naturalistic observation,
you can start making sense out of it
and even predict future behavior.
One way to do that is to look at
how one trait or behavior is related
to another or how they correlate.
So let's get back to my buddy Bob,
who seems to think that his refrigerator
is actually some kind of time machine
that can preserve food indefinitely.
Let's say that Bob is just tucked into
a lunch of questionable leftovers...
Pizza that may very well have
had a little bit of fungus on it...
But he was hungry. And lazy.
And so he doused it in sriracha.
Suddenly, he starts seeing things.
Green armadillos with laser-beam-eyes.
From here we can deduce that eating
unknown fungus predicts hallucination.
That's a correlation;
but correlation is not causation.
Yes, it makes sense that eating questionable
fungus would cause hallucinations,
but it's possible that Bob was already
on the verge of a psychotic episode
and those fuzzy left-overs were actually benign!
Or, they could be an entirely
different factor involved,
like maybe he hadn't slept in 72 hours
or had an intense migraine coming on,
and one of those factors
caused his hallucinations.
It's tempting to draw
conclusions from correlations,
but it's super important to remember
that correlations predict the POSSIBILITY
of a cause-and-effect relationships;
they can not prove them.
So we've talked about how to describe
behavior without manipulating it,
and how to make connections and
predictions from those findings,
but that can only take you so far.
To really get to the bottom of
cause-and-effect behaviors,
you're gonna have to start experimenting.
Experiments allow investigators
to isolate different effects
by manipulating an independent variable
and keeping all other variables constant
(or as constant as you can).
This means that they need at least two groups:
the experimental group, which is
gonna get "messed with";
and the control group, which is
not going to get "messed with".
Just as surveys use random samples,
experimental researchers need to
randomly assign participants to each group
to minimize potential confounding variables
or outside factors that may skew the results.
You don't want all grumpy teenagers in one group
and wealthy Japanese servers
in the other; they gotta mingle.
Sometimes one or both groups are not
informed about what's actually being tested.
For example, researchers can test
how substances affect people
by comparing their effects to
placebos, or inert substances.
And often, the researchers themselves
don't know which group is experimental
and which is control,
so they don't unintentionally influence
the results through their own behavior.
In which case, it's called...
You guessed it! A double-blind procedure.
So let's put these ideas into practice
in our own little experiment.
Like all good work, it starts with a question.
So the other day, my friend
Bernice and I were debating.
We were debating coffee
and its effect on the brain.
Personally, she's convinced that coffee
helps her focus and think better,
but I get all jittery, like a caged meerkat
and can't focus on anything.
And because we know that over-confidence
can lead to belief that are not true,
we decided to do some critical thinking.
So let's figure out our question:
"Do humans solve problems
faster when given caffeine?"
Now we've got to boil that down
into a testable prediction.
Remember: Keep it clear, simple, and
eloquent so that it can be replicated.
"Caffeine makes me smarter"
is not a great hypothesis.
A better one would be, say...
"Adults humans given caffeine will navigate
a maze faster than humans not given caffeine."
The caffeine dosage is your independent
variable (the thing that you can change).
So, you'll need some coffee.
Your result or dependent variable--
(the thing that depends on
the thing that you can change),
is going to be the speed at which the
subject navigates this giant corn maze.
Go out on the street, wrangle up a
bunch of different kinds of people,
and randomly assign them
into three different groups.
Also at this point, the American
Psychological Association suggests
that you acquire everyone's
informed consent to participate.
You don't want to force anyone
to be in your experiment,
no matter how cool you think it is.
So the control group gets
a placebo (in this case, decaf).
Experimental group 1 gets
a low dose of caffeine,
which we'll define at 100mg
(just an eye opener,
like a cup of coffee's worth).
Experimental group 2 gets 500 mg
(more than a quad-shot of espresso
dumped in a Red Bull).
Once you dose everyone,
turn them loose in the maze
and wait at the other end with a stopwatch.
All that's left is to measure your
results from the three different groups
and compare them, just to see if
there were any conclusive results.
If the highly-dosed folks got through it
twice as fast as the low dose, placebo groups
then Bernice's hypothesis was correct
and she can rub my face in it,
saying she was right all along,
but really, that would just be
the warm flush of hindsight bias
telling her something she didn't
really know until we tested it.
Then, because we've used clear
language in defining our parameters,
other curious minds can easily
replicate this experiment and
we can eventually pool all the data together,
and have something solid to say about what
that macchiato was doing to your cognition.
Or at least the speed at which
you can run through a maze.
Science!
Probably the best tool that you have
for understanding other people.
Thanks for watching this episode
of Crash Course Psychology!
If you've paid attention,
you've learned how to apply the scientific
method to psychological research
through case studies, naturalistic observation,
surveys and interviews, and experimentation.
You've also learned about different
kinds of bias in experimentation,
and how research practices help us avoid them.
Thanks specially to our Subbable subscribers,
who make this and all of Crash Course possible.
If you'd like to contribute to help us keep
Crash Course going and also get awesome perks,
like an autographed science poster or
even be animated into an upcoming episode
go to subbable.com/crashcourse to find out how.
[Host reads the credits]