WEBVTT 00:00:14.000 --> 00:00:18.000 Take a series of still, sequential images. 00:00:18.000 --> 00:00:20.000 Let's look at them one by one. 00:00:23.000 --> 00:00:24.000 Faster. 00:00:28.000 --> 00:00:30.000 Now, let's remove the gaps, 00:00:30.000 --> 00:00:31.000 go faster still. 00:00:32.000 --> 00:00:34.000 Wait for it... 00:00:36.000 --> 00:00:37.000 ...bam! 00:00:37.000 --> 00:00:38.000 Motion! 00:00:39.000 --> 00:00:41.000 Why is that? 00:00:41.000 --> 00:00:42.000 Intellectually, we know we're just looking 00:00:42.000 --> 00:00:44.000 at a series of still images, 00:00:44.000 --> 00:00:46.000 but when we see them change fast enough, 00:00:46.000 --> 00:00:47.000 they produce the optical illusion 00:00:47.000 --> 00:00:49.000 of appearing as a single, persistent image 00:00:49.000 --> 00:00:52.000 that's gradually changing form and position. 00:00:52.000 --> 00:00:56.000 This effect is the basis for all motion picture technology, 00:00:56.000 --> 00:00:58.000 from our LED screens of today 00:00:58.000 --> 00:01:00.000 to their 20th century cathode ray forebears, 00:01:00.000 --> 00:01:02.000 from cinematic film projection 00:01:02.000 --> 00:01:03.000 to the novelty toy, 00:01:03.000 --> 00:01:05.000 even, it's been suggested, 00:01:05.000 --> 00:01:06.000 all the way back to the Stone Age 00:01:06.000 --> 00:01:09.000 when humans began painting on cave walls. 00:01:09.000 --> 00:01:11.000 This phenomenon of perceiving apparent motion 00:01:11.000 --> 00:01:13.000 in successive images 00:01:13.000 --> 00:01:15.000 is due to a characteristic of human perception 00:01:15.000 --> 00:01:18.000 historically referred to as "persistence of vision". 00:01:18.000 --> 00:01:19.000 The term is attributed 00:01:19.000 --> 00:01:22.000 to the English-Swiss physicist Peter Mark Roget, 00:01:22.000 --> 00:01:24.000 who, in the early 19th century, 00:01:24.000 --> 00:01:27.000 used it to describe a particular defect of the eye 00:01:27.000 --> 00:01:28.000 that resulted in a moving object 00:01:28.000 --> 00:01:32.000 appearing to be still when it reached a certain speed. 00:01:32.000 --> 00:01:33.000 Not long after, 00:01:33.000 --> 00:01:35.000 the term was applied to describe the opposite, 00:01:35.000 --> 00:01:37.000 the apparent motion of still images, 00:01:37.000 --> 00:01:39.000 by Belgian physicist Joseph Plateau, 00:01:39.000 --> 00:01:41.000 inventor of the phenakistoscope. 00:01:41.000 --> 00:01:43.000 He defined persistence of vision 00:01:43.000 --> 00:01:46.000 as the result of successive afterimages, 00:01:46.000 --> 00:01:48.000 which were retained and then combined in the retina, 00:01:48.000 --> 00:01:50.000 making us believe that what we were seeing 00:01:50.000 --> 00:01:52.000 is a single object in motion. 00:01:52.000 --> 00:01:54.000 This explanation was widely accepted 00:01:54.000 --> 00:01:55.000 in the decades to follow 00:01:55.000 --> 00:01:57.000 and up through the turn of the 20th century, 00:01:57.000 --> 00:01:58.000 when some began to question 00:01:58.000 --> 00:02:01.000 what was physiologically going on. 00:02:01.000 --> 00:02:04.000 In 1912, German psychologist Max Wertheimer 00:02:04.000 --> 00:02:06.000 outlined the basic primary stages of apparent motion 00:02:06.000 --> 00:02:09.000 using simple optical illusions. 00:02:09.000 --> 00:02:10.000 These experiments led him to conclude 00:02:10.000 --> 00:02:12.000 the phenomenon was due to processes 00:02:12.000 --> 00:02:15.000 which lie behind the retina. 00:02:15.000 --> 00:02:17.000 In 1915, Hugo Munsterberg, 00:02:17.000 --> 00:02:19.000 a German-American pioneer in applied psychology, 00:02:19.000 --> 00:02:21.000 also suggested that the apparent motion 00:02:21.000 --> 00:02:22.000 of successive images 00:02:22.000 --> 00:02:25.000 is not due to their being retained in the eye, 00:02:25.000 --> 00:02:28.000 but is superadded by the action of the mind. 00:02:29.000 --> 00:02:30.000 In the century to follow, 00:02:30.000 --> 00:02:31.000 experiments by physiologists 00:02:31.000 --> 00:02:34.000 have pretty much confirm their conclusions. 00:02:34.000 --> 00:02:36.000 As it relates to the illusion of motion pictures, 00:02:36.000 --> 00:02:39.000 persistence of vision has less to do with vision itself 00:02:39.000 --> 00:02:41.000 than how it's interpreted in the brain. 00:02:41.000 --> 00:02:43.000 Research has shown that different aspects 00:02:43.000 --> 00:02:45.000 of what the eye sees, 00:02:45.000 --> 00:02:45.000 like form, 00:02:45.000 --> 00:02:46.000 color, 00:02:46.000 --> 00:02:47.000 depth, 00:02:47.000 --> 00:02:48.000 and motion, 00:02:48.000 --> 00:02:51.000 are transmitted to different areas of the visual cortex 00:02:51.000 --> 00:02:53.000 via different pathways from the retina. 00:02:53.000 --> 00:02:54.000 It's the continuous interaction 00:02:54.000 --> 00:02:56.000 of various computations in the visual cortex 00:02:56.000 --> 00:02:58.000 that stitch those different aspects together 00:02:58.000 --> 00:03:01.000 and culminate in the perception. 00:03:01.000 --> 00:03:02.000 Our brains are constantly working, 00:03:02.000 --> 00:03:04.000 synchronizing what we see, 00:03:04.000 --> 00:03:04.000 hear, 00:03:04.000 --> 00:03:05.000 smell, 00:03:05.000 --> 00:03:05.000 and touch 00:03:05.000 --> 00:03:06.000 into meaningful experience 00:03:06.000 --> 00:03:09.000 in the moment-to-moment flow of the present. 00:03:09.000 --> 00:03:10.000 So, in order to create the illusion 00:03:10.000 --> 00:03:12.000 of motion in successive images, 00:03:12.000 --> 00:03:14.000 you need to get the timing of our intervals 00:03:14.000 --> 00:03:17.000 close to the speed at which our brains process the present. 00:03:18.000 --> 00:03:21.000 So, fast is the present happening according to our brains? 00:03:21.000 --> 00:03:22.000 Well, we can get an idea 00:03:22.000 --> 00:03:24.000 by measuring how fast the images need to be changing 00:03:24.000 --> 00:03:26.000 for the illusion to work. 00:03:26.000 --> 00:03:27.000 Let's see if we can figure it out 00:03:27.000 --> 00:03:29.000 by repeating our experiment. 00:03:29.000 --> 00:03:31.000 Here's the sequence presented 00:03:31.000 --> 00:03:33.000 at the rate of one frame per two seconds 00:03:33.000 --> 00:03:36.000 with one second of black in-between. 00:03:36.000 --> 00:03:37.000 At this rate of change 00:03:37.000 --> 00:03:39.000 with the blank space separating the images, 00:03:39.000 --> 00:03:41.000 there's no real motion perceptible. 00:03:41.000 --> 00:03:44.000 As we lessen the duration of blank space, 00:03:44.000 --> 00:03:46.000 a slight change in position becomes more apparent, 00:03:46.000 --> 00:03:48.000 and you start to get an inkling of a sense of motion 00:03:48.000 --> 00:03:50.000 between the disparate frames. 00:03:50.000 --> 00:03:53.000 One frame per second, 00:03:55.000 --> 00:03:57.000 two frames per second, 00:03:59.000 --> 00:04:01.000 four frames per second. 00:04:02.000 --> 00:04:04.000 Now we're starting to get a feeling of motion, 00:04:04.000 --> 00:04:06.000 but it's really not very smooth. 00:04:06.000 --> 00:04:07.000 We're still aware of the fact 00:04:07.000 --> 00:04:09.000 that we're looking at separate images. 00:04:09.000 --> 00:04:10.000 Let's speed up, 00:04:10.000 --> 00:04:12.000 eight frames per second, 00:04:13.000 --> 00:04:15.000 twelve frames per second. 00:04:16.000 --> 00:04:18.000 It looks like we're about there. 00:04:21.000 --> 00:04:22.000 At twenty-four frames per second, 00:04:22.000 --> 00:04:24.000 the motion looks even smoother. 00:04:24.000 --> 00:04:26.000 This is standard full speed. 00:04:28.000 --> 00:04:30.000 So the point at which we loose awareness of the intervals 00:04:30.000 --> 00:04:31.000 and begin to see apparent motion 00:04:31.000 --> 00:04:35.000 seems to kick in at around eight to twelve frames per second. 00:04:35.000 --> 00:04:36.000 This is in the neighborhood 00:04:36.000 --> 00:04:37.000 of what science has determined 00:04:37.000 --> 00:04:39.000 to be the general threshold of our awareness 00:04:39.000 --> 00:04:41.000 of seeing separate images. 00:04:41.000 --> 00:04:43.000 Generally speaking, we being to lose that awareness 00:04:43.000 --> 00:04:46.000 at intervals of around 100 milliseconds per image, 00:04:46.000 --> 00:04:48.000 which is equal to a frame rate of 00:04:48.000 --> 00:04:50.000 around ten frames per second. 00:04:50.000 --> 00:04:51.000 As the frame rate increases, 00:04:51.000 --> 00:04:53.000 we lose awareness of the intervals completely 00:04:53.000 --> 00:04:54.000 and are all the more convinced 00:04:54.000 --> 00:04:56.000 of the reality of the illusion.