Return to Video

What do all languages have in common? - Cameron Morin

  • 0:07 - 0:10
    Language is endlessly variable.
  • 0:10 - 0:13
    Each of us can come up with
    an infinite number of sentences
  • 0:13 - 0:14
    in our native language,
  • 0:14 - 0:17
    and we’re able to do so from an early age—
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    almost as soon as we start
    to communicate in sentences.
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    How is this possible?
  • 0:22 - 0:26
    In the early 1950s, Noam Chomsky
    proposed a theory
  • 0:26 - 0:31
    based on the observation that the key
    to this versatility seems to be grammar:
  • 0:31 - 0:35
    the familiar grammatical structure
    of an unfamiliar sentence
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    points us toward its meaning.
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    He suggested that there are
    grammatical rules
  • 0:40 - 0:45
    that apply to all languages,
    and that the rules are innate—
  • 0:45 - 0:50
    the human brain is hardwired to process
    language according to these rules.
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    He labelled this faculty
    universal grammar,
  • 0:53 - 0:57
    and it launched lines of inquiry
    that shaped both the field of linguistics
  • 0:57 - 1:02
    and the emerging field
    of cognitive science for decades to come.
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    Chomsky and other researchers
    set out to investigate
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    the two main components
    of universal grammar:
  • 1:08 - 1:11
    first, whether there are, in fact,
    grammar rules
  • 1:11 - 1:13
    that are universal to all languages,
  • 1:13 - 1:18
    and, second, whether these rules
    are hardwired in the brain.
  • 1:18 - 1:21
    In attempts to establish
    the universal rules of grammar,
  • 1:21 - 1:26
    Chomsky developed an analytical tool
    known as generative syntax,
  • 1:26 - 1:32
    which represents the order of words
    in a sentence in hierarchical syntax trees
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    that show what structures are possible.
  • 1:34 - 1:38
    Based on this tree, we could suggest
    a grammar rule
  • 1:38 - 1:41
    that adverbs must occur in verb phrases.
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    But with more data,
    it quickly becomes clear
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    that adverbs can appear
    outside of verb phrases.
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    This simplified example illustrates
    a major problem:
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    it takes a lot of data
    from each individual language
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    to establish the rules for that language,
  • 1:57 - 1:59
    before we can even begin to determine
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    which rules all languages
    might have in common.
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    When Chomsky proposed universal grammar,
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    many languages lacked the volume
    of recorded samples
  • 2:09 - 2:12
    necessary to analyze them
    using generative syntax.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    Even with lots of data,
  • 2:14 - 2:18
    mapping the structure of a language
    is incredibly complex.
  • 2:18 - 2:24
    After 50 years of analysis, we still
    haven’t completely figured out English.
  • 2:24 - 2:27
    As more linguist data
    was gathered and analyzed,
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    it became clear that languages
    around the world differ widely,
  • 2:31 - 2:35
    challenging the theory that there were
    universal grammar rules.
  • 2:35 - 2:39
    In the 1980s, Chomsky revised his theory
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    in an attempt to accommodate
    this variation.
  • 2:41 - 2:46
    According to his new hypothesis
    of principles and parameters,
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    all languages shared certain
    grammatical principles,
  • 2:49 - 2:53
    but could vary in their parameters,
    or the application of these principles.
  • 2:53 - 2:58
    For example, a principle is
    “every sentence must have a subject,"
  • 2:58 - 3:02
    but the parameter of whether the subject
    must be explicitly stated
  • 3:02 - 3:04
    could vary between languages.
  • 3:04 - 3:06
    The hypothesis of principles
    and parameters
  • 3:06 - 3:11
    still didn’t answer the question of which
    grammatical principles are universal.
  • 3:11 - 3:16
    In the early 2000s, Chomsky suggested
    that there’s just one shared principle,
  • 3:16 - 3:22
    called recursion, which means structures
    can be nested inside each other.
  • 3:22 - 3:23
    Take this sentence,
  • 3:23 - 3:27
    which embeds a sentence within a sentence
    within a sentence.
  • 3:27 - 3:31
    Or this sentence, which embeds
    a noun phrase in a noun phrase
  • 3:31 - 3:32
    in a noun phrase.
  • 3:32 - 3:36
    Recursion was a good candidate
    for a universal grammar rule
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    because it can take many forms.
  • 3:38 - 3:42
    However, in 2005 linguists
    published findings
  • 3:42 - 3:45
    on an Amazonian language called Piraha,
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    which doesn’t appear to have
    any recursive structures.
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    So what about the other part
    of Chomsky’s theory,
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    that our language faculty is innate?
  • 3:55 - 3:58
    When he first proposed universal grammar,
  • 3:58 - 4:02
    the idea that there was a genetically
    determined aspect of language acquisition
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    had a profound, revolutionary impact.
  • 4:05 - 4:10
    It challenged the dominant paradigm,
    called behaviorism.
  • 4:10 - 4:15
    Behaviorists argued that all animal
    and human behaviors, including language,
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    were acquired from the outside
    by the mind,
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    which starts out as a blank slate.
  • 4:21 - 4:25
    Today, scientists agree that behaviorism
    was wrong,
  • 4:25 - 4:29
    and there is underlying,
    genetically encoded biological machinery
  • 4:29 - 4:30
    for language learning.
  • 4:30 - 4:33
    Many think the same biology
    responsible for language
  • 4:33 - 4:37
    is also responsible for other
    aspects of cognition.
  • 4:37 - 4:40
    So they disagree with Chomsky’s idea
  • 4:40 - 4:45
    that there is a specific, isolated,
    innate language faculty in the brain.
  • 4:45 - 4:49
    The theory of universal grammar
    prompted the documentation and study
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    of many languages
    that hadn’t been studied before.
  • 4:52 - 4:57
    It also caused an old idea to be
    reevaluated and eventually overthrown
  • 4:57 - 5:01
    to make room for our growing
    understanding of the human brain.
Title:
What do all languages have in common? - Cameron Morin
Speaker:
Cameron Morin
Description:

View full lesson: https://ed.ted.com/lessons/what-do-all-languages-have-in-common-cameron-morin

Language is endlessly variable. Each of us can come up with an infinite number of sentences in our native language, and we’re able to do so from an early age— almost as soon as we start to communicate in sentences. How is this possible? In the early 1950s, Noam Chomsky proposed a theory that the key to this versatility was grammar. Cameron Morin details Chomsky's theory of universal grammar.

Lesson by Cameron Morin, directed by Eoin Duffy.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
05:02

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions