Return to Video

What crows teach us about death

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    Whether we want to or not,
    humans spend a great deal of time
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    considering death.
  • 0:06 - 0:10
    And it's possible we've been doing so
    since shortly after homo sapiens
  • 0:10 - 0:13
    first began roaming the landscape.
  • 0:13 - 0:15
    After all, the first
    intentional human burial
  • 0:15 - 0:19
    is thought to have occurred
    around 100,000 years ago.
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    What might those early people
    have been thinking
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    as they took the time
    to dig into the earth,
  • 0:26 - 0:27
    deposit the body
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    and carefully cover it up again?
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    Were they trying
    to protect it from scavengers
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    or stymie the spread of disease?
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    Were they trying to honor the deceased?
  • 0:38 - 0:42
    Or did they just not want
    to have to look at a dead body?
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    Without the advent of a time machine,
  • 0:44 - 0:48
    we may never know for sure
    what those early people were thinking,
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    but one thing we do know
    is that humans are far from alone
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    in our attention towards the dead.
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    Like people, some animals,
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    including the corvids, the family of birds
  • 0:58 - 1:02
    that houses the crows,
    ravens, magpies and jays,
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    also seem to pay
    special attention to their dead.
  • 1:05 - 1:09
    In fact, the rituals of corvids
    may have acted as the inspiration
  • 1:09 - 1:10
    for our own.
  • 1:10 - 1:13
    After all, it was the raven
    that God sent down
  • 1:13 - 1:17
    to teach Cain how to bury
    his slain brother Abel.
  • 1:17 - 1:21
    But despite this clear recognition
    by early people that other animals
  • 1:21 - 1:22
    attend to their dead,
  • 1:22 - 1:26
    it's only fairly recently that science
    has really turned its attention
  • 1:26 - 1:28
    towards this phenomenon.
  • 1:28 - 1:32
    In fact, a formal name for this field --
    comparative thanatology --
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    wasn't first introduced until 2016.
  • 1:35 - 1:39
    In this growing field, we are beginning
    to appreciate what a rich place
  • 1:39 - 1:44
    the natural world is with respect to how
    other animals interact with their dead,
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    and it's in this growing body of knowledge
  • 1:46 - 1:50
    that that time machine
    to our early ancestors might be possible.
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    So what are we learning
    in this growing field?
  • 1:55 - 1:59
    Well, right now we can split
    our understanding into two main groups.
  • 1:59 - 2:04
    In the first, we have animals that display
    stereotyped, predictable behaviors
  • 2:04 - 2:05
    towards their dead,
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    and for whom much
    of what we understand about them
  • 2:07 - 2:10
    comes from experimental studies.
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    This group includes things
    like the social insects --
  • 2:13 - 2:15
    bees and ants and termites --
  • 2:15 - 2:19
    and for all of these animals,
    colony hygiene is of critical importance,
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    and so as a result these animals
    display rigorous undertaking behaviors
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    in response to corpses.
  • 2:25 - 2:29
    For example, they may physically
    remove carcasses from the colony.
  • 2:29 - 2:30
    They may consume them.
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    They may even construct tombs.
  • 2:33 - 2:37
    We see similar hygiene-driven responses
    in some colony-living mammals.
  • 2:37 - 2:40
    Rats, for example,
    will reliably bury cage-mates
  • 2:40 - 2:43
    that have been dead for 48 hours.
  • 2:44 - 2:48
    In our other group, we have animals
    that display more variable,
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    perhaps more charismatic behaviors,
  • 2:50 - 2:53
    and for whom much
    of what we understand about them
  • 2:53 - 2:54
    comes from anecdotes
  • 2:54 - 2:57
    by scientists or other observers.
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    This is the animals whose death behaviors
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    I suspect might be more familiar to folks.
  • 3:02 - 3:05
    It includes organisms like elephants,
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    which are well-known
    for their attendance to their dead,
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    even in popular culture.
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    In fact, they're even known
    to be attracted
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    to the bones of their deceased.
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    It also includes animals like primates,
  • 3:16 - 3:19
    which display a wide variety
    of behaviors around their dead,
  • 3:19 - 3:21
    from grooming them
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    to prolonged attention towards them,
  • 3:25 - 3:26
    guarding them,
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    even the transportation of dead infants.
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    And that's actually a behavior
    we've seen in a number of animals,
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    like the dolphins for example.
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    You may remember the story of Tahlequah,
  • 3:37 - 3:40
    the orca in the resident J pod
    in the Puget Sound,
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    who during the summer of 2018
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    carried her dead calf
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    for an unprecedented 17 days.
  • 3:48 - 3:49
    Now, a story like that
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    is both heartbreaking and fascinating,
  • 3:52 - 3:56
    but it offers far more questions
    than it does answers.
  • 3:56 - 3:59
    For example, why
    did Tahlequah carry her calf
  • 3:59 - 4:01
    for such a long period of time?
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    Was she just that stricken with grief?
  • 4:04 - 4:08
    Was she more confused
    by her unresponsive infant?
  • 4:08 - 4:11
    Or is this behavior
    just less rare in orcas
  • 4:11 - 4:13
    than we currently understand it to be?
  • 4:14 - 4:15
    But for a variety of reasons,
  • 4:15 - 4:19
    it's difficult to do the kinds
    of experimental studies
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    in an animal like an orca,
    or many of these other large mammals,
  • 4:22 - 4:25
    that might elucidate
    those kinds of questions.
  • 4:25 - 4:29
    So instead, science is turning
    to an animal whose behaviors around death
  • 4:29 - 4:31
    we've been thinking about since BCE:
  • 4:32 - 4:34
    the crows.
  • 4:34 - 4:35
    Like insects and primates,
  • 4:35 - 4:39
    crows also seem to pay
    special attention to their dead.
  • 4:39 - 4:42
    Typically, this manifests
    as the discovering bird alarm calling,
  • 4:42 - 4:44
    like you can see in this photo,
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    followed by the recruitment
    of other birds to the area
  • 4:47 - 4:48
    to form what we call a mob.
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    But it can be a little
    different than that too.
  • 4:52 - 4:57
    For example, I've had people share with me
    seeing prolonged silent vigils by crows
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    in response to deceased or dying crows.
  • 5:00 - 5:04
    I've even had people tell me
    of witnessing crows place objects
  • 5:04 - 5:08
    like sticks and candy wrappers
    on or near the bodies of dead crows.
  • 5:09 - 5:12
    And this mix of observations
    puts these birds
  • 5:12 - 5:14
    in a really important place in our scheme,
  • 5:14 - 5:18
    because it suggests on the one hand
    they might be like the insects,
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    displaying these
    very predictable behaviors,
  • 5:21 - 5:24
    but on the other hand
    we have this handful of observations
  • 5:24 - 5:28
    that are more difficult to explain
    and feel a bit more like what we see
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    in some of the mammals
    like primates and elephants.
  • 5:31 - 5:36
    And like those animals, crows share
    an extremely large relative brain size
  • 5:36 - 5:40
    and the kinds of dynamic social lives
    that might invite more complexity
  • 5:40 - 5:42
    in how they respond to their dead.
  • 5:42 - 5:46
    So I wanted to try to understand
    what was going on
  • 5:46 - 5:49
    when crows encounter a dead crow,
  • 5:49 - 5:52
    and what this might teach us
    about the role of death in their world,
  • 5:52 - 5:55
    and possibly the worlds
    of other animals as well,
  • 5:55 - 5:57
    even those early versions of ourselves.
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    There's a number of different ways
    that we could explain
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    why crows might be
    attracted to their dead.
  • 6:04 - 6:08
    For example, maybe
    it's a social opportunity,
  • 6:08 - 6:11
    a way for them to explore
    why that individual died,
  • 6:11 - 6:13
    who they were
  • 6:13 - 6:17
    and what impact this is going to have
    on the neighborhood moving forward.
  • 6:17 - 6:18
    Maybe it's an expression of grief,
  • 6:18 - 6:20
    like our own contemporary funerals.
  • 6:21 - 6:25
    Or maybe it's a way that they learn
    about danger in their environment.
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    While all of those explanations
    are worth pursuing,
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    and certainly not mutually exclusive,
  • 6:31 - 6:34
    they're not all testable
    scientific questions.
  • 6:34 - 6:40
    But that idea that dead crows
    might act as cues of danger, that is.
  • 6:40 - 6:43
    So as a graduate student,
    I wanted to explore that question,
  • 6:43 - 6:47
    particularly with respect to two ideas.
  • 6:47 - 6:51
    The first was whether they
    might be able to learn new predators,
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    specifically people,
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    based on their association
    with dead crows.
  • 6:56 - 6:58
    And the second was
    if they might learn places
  • 6:58 - 7:03
    associated with where
    they find crow bodies.
  • 7:03 - 7:07
    So to do this, I would go out into
    some unsuspecting Seattle neighborhood
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    and I would start to feed
    a breeding pair of crows
  • 7:10 - 7:12
    over the course of three days,
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    and this provided a baseline
  • 7:14 - 7:17
    for how quickly the crows
    would come down to a food pile,
  • 7:17 - 7:19
    which, as you'll see in a minute,
    was really important.
  • 7:20 - 7:21
    Then, on the fourth day,
  • 7:21 - 7:22
    we would have our funeral.
  • 7:24 - 7:25
    This is Linda.
  • 7:25 - 7:29
    Linda is one of seven masks whose job
    was to stand there for 30 minutes
  • 7:29 - 7:32
    with her little hors d'oeuvre
    plate of dead crow
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    while I documented what happened.
  • 7:35 - 7:36
    Most importantly, though,
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    her job was to come back after a week,
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    now without the dead crow,
  • 7:40 - 7:44
    so that we could see if the birds would
    treat her just like any old pedestrian,
  • 7:44 - 7:48
    or if, instead, they would exhibit
    behaviors like alarm calling
  • 7:48 - 7:50
    or dive bombing
  • 7:50 - 7:53
    that would indicate
    that they perceived her as a predator.
  • 7:54 - 7:57
    Now, given that we already knew
    crows were capable of learning
  • 7:57 - 7:59
    and recognizing human faces,
  • 7:59 - 8:03
    it may come as no surprise
    that the majority of crows in our study
  • 8:03 - 8:07
    did treat the masks that they saw
    handling dead crows as threats
  • 8:07 - 8:10
    when they saw them over the course
    of the next six weeks.
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    Now, if you're sitting there thinking,
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    alright, give me a break,
  • 8:14 - 8:17
    look at that face, it is terrifying,
  • 8:17 - 8:19
    anyone would treat that as a threat
  • 8:19 - 8:21
    if they saw it walking down the street,
  • 8:21 - 8:23
    know that you are not alone.
  • 8:23 - 8:25
    As it turns out, a lot of the folks
  • 8:25 - 8:27
    whose houses we did
    these experiments in front of
  • 8:27 - 8:29
    felt the same way,
  • 8:29 - 8:31
    but we'll save that for another time.
  • 8:31 - 8:34
    So you may be comforted to know
    that we did control tests
  • 8:34 - 8:39
    to make sure that crows don't share
    our preconceived bias against masks
  • 8:39 - 8:42
    that look a bit like
    the female version of Hannibal Lecter.
  • 8:43 - 8:47
    Now, in addition to finding that crows
    were able to make associations with people
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    based on their handling of dead crows,
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    we also found that in the days
    following these funeral events,
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    as we continued to feed them,
  • 8:55 - 8:59
    that their willingness to come down
    to the food pile significantly diminished,
  • 8:59 - 9:03
    and we didn't see that same kind
    of decline in our control groups.
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    So that suggests that, yes,
    crows can make associations
  • 9:06 - 9:10
    with particular places
    where they've seen dead crows.
  • 9:10 - 9:12
    So together, what that tells us is that
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    while we certainly shouldn't discount
    those other explanations,
  • 9:16 - 9:18
    we can feel pretty confident in saying
  • 9:18 - 9:21
    that for crows, attention to their dead
  • 9:21 - 9:22
    might be a really important way
  • 9:22 - 9:25
    that these animals learn about danger.
  • 9:25 - 9:28
    And that's a nice, tidy little narrative
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    on which to hang our hats.
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    But in life and death,
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    things are rarely so neat,
  • 9:34 - 9:38
    and I really came face to face with that
    in a follow-up experiment,
  • 9:38 - 9:41
    where we were looking at
    how crows respond to dead crows
  • 9:41 - 9:44
    in the absence of any kind of predator.
  • 9:44 - 9:47
    And suffice it to say,
    we found that in these cases,
  • 9:47 - 9:50
    the wakes can get a little more weird.
  • 9:51 - 9:54
    So this is what that
    experimental setup looks like.
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    You can see our stuffed dead crow
    alone on the sidewalk,
  • 9:56 - 10:00
    and it's been placed
    on the territory of a pair.
  • 10:00 - 10:01
    (Squawk)
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    That is the alarm call
    by one of those territorial birds,
  • 10:04 - 10:05
    and it's coming into frame.
  • 10:06 - 10:09
    Pretty soon, its mate is going to join it.
  • 10:12 - 10:15
    And so far, this is all very usual.
  • 10:15 - 10:16
    This is what crows do.
  • 10:16 - 10:19
    OK, right now it's getting
    a little less usual.
  • 10:20 - 10:25
    Not everyone here might be familiar
    with what bird sex looks like,
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    so if you are not,
    this is what it looks like.
  • 10:33 - 10:37
    You're basically seeing
    a confluence of three behaviors:
  • 10:37 - 10:41
    alarm, as indicated by the alarm calling;
  • 10:41 - 10:44
    aggression, as indicated
    by the very forceful pecking
  • 10:44 - 10:50
    by both one of the copulatory birds
    and one of the excited bystanders;
  • 10:50 - 10:51
    and sexual arousal.
  • 10:51 - 10:54
    Clearly, this is startling,
  • 10:54 - 10:58
    and interesting to think about
    and talk about.
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    But if our goal is to understand
  • 11:00 - 11:04
    the big picture of how animals
    interact with their dead,
  • 11:04 - 11:08
    then the most important question
    we should ask is, is this representative?
  • 11:08 - 11:11
    Is this something
    that's happening consistently?
  • 11:11 - 11:14
    And that's why being able to do
    systematic studies with crows
  • 11:14 - 11:16
    is so valuable,
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    because after conducting
    hundreds of these trials,
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    where I was placing these dead crows
    out on the sidewalks
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    on the territories
    of hundreds of different pairs,
  • 11:24 - 11:27
    what we found was that, no, it's not.
  • 11:27 - 11:28
    Contact of any kind,
  • 11:28 - 11:31
    whether it was sexual, aggressive
  • 11:31 - 11:32
    or even just exploratory,
  • 11:32 - 11:35
    only occurred 30 percent of the time.
  • 11:35 - 11:39
    So given that this wasn't representative,
  • 11:39 - 11:40
    this was the minority,
  • 11:40 - 11:42
    we may be tempted to just dismiss it
  • 11:42 - 11:46
    as irrelevant, odd, creepy,
    weird crow behavior.
  • 11:46 - 11:49
    But what may surprise you
    is that behaviors like aggression
  • 11:50 - 11:52
    or even sexual arousal
  • 11:52 - 11:53
    aren't all that rare,
  • 11:53 - 11:56
    and certainly aren't
    constrained to just crows.
  • 11:56 - 12:00
    Because while the popular narrative
    when it comes to animal death behaviors
  • 12:00 - 12:03
    tends to focus on affiliative behaviors
  • 12:03 - 12:05
    like grooming or guarding,
  • 12:05 - 12:10
    that is far from the complete list
    of what even our closest relatives do
  • 12:10 - 12:12
    around their dead.
  • 12:12 - 12:18
    In fact, we've documented behaviors
    like biting, beating and even sex itself
  • 12:18 - 12:19
    in a wide variety of animals,
  • 12:19 - 12:22
    including many primates and dolphins.
  • 12:24 - 12:27
    So where does this leave us
    in our understanding of animals
  • 12:27 - 12:28
    and their death rituals?
  • 12:29 - 12:32
    Well, for crows, it suggests that,
  • 12:32 - 12:36
    like insects, they may have
    a strong adaptive driver
  • 12:36 - 12:37
    in their interest in their dead.
  • 12:37 - 12:40
    In this case, it might be danger learning,
  • 12:40 - 12:42
    and that might have acted
    as the inspiration
  • 12:42 - 12:44
    for our own rituals as well.
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    But when we look more closely,
  • 12:47 - 12:50
    we see that there's
    no one simple narrative
  • 12:50 - 12:53
    that can explain
    the vast array of behaviors
  • 12:53 - 12:55
    we see in crows and many other animals.
  • 12:56 - 13:01
    And that suggests that we are still
    far from completing that time machine.
  • 13:01 - 13:05
    But it's going to be
    a really fascinating ride.
  • 13:05 - 13:07
    Thank you.
  • 13:07 - 13:10
    (Applause)
Title:
What crows teach us about death
Speaker:
Kaeli Swift
Description:

Rituals for the dead span much of the natural world, seen in practices from humans and elephants to bees, dolphins and beyond. With charm and playful insight, animal behaviorist Kaeli Swift delves into the life (and death) habits of crows and shares what their responses could reveal about our own relationship to mortality.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
13:24
Erin Gregory approved English subtitles for What crows teach us about death
Erin Gregory edited English subtitles for What crows teach us about death
Joanna Pietrulewicz accepted English subtitles for What crows teach us about death
Joanna Pietrulewicz edited English subtitles for What crows teach us about death
Joanna Pietrulewicz edited English subtitles for What crows teach us about death
Joseph Geni edited English subtitles for What crows teach us about death
Joseph Geni edited English subtitles for What crows teach us about death

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions