Return to Video

Moral behavior in animals

  • 0:00 - 0:02
    I was born in Den Bosch,
  • 0:02 - 0:05
    where the painter Hieronymus Bosch named himself after.
  • 0:05 - 0:07
    And so I've always been very fond of this painter
  • 0:07 - 0:10
    who lived and worked in the 15th century.
  • 0:10 - 0:12
    And what is interesting about him in relation to morality
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    is that he lived at a time where religion's influence was waning,
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    and he was sort of wondering, I think,
  • 0:17 - 0:19
    what would happen with society
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    if there was no religion or if there was less religion.
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    And so he painted this famous painting, "The Garden of Earthly Delights,"
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    which some have interpreted
  • 0:27 - 0:29
    as being humanity before the Fall,
  • 0:29 - 0:32
    or being humanity without any Fall at all.
  • 0:32 - 0:34
    And so it makes you wonder,
  • 0:34 - 0:37
    what would happen if we hadn't tasted the fruit of knowledge, so to speak,
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    and what kind of morality would we have?
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    Much later, as a student,
  • 0:42 - 0:44
    I went to a very different garden,
  • 0:44 - 0:47
    a zoological garden in Arnhem
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    where we keep chimpanzees.
  • 0:49 - 0:51
    This is me at an early age with a baby chimpanzee.
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    (Laughter)
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    And I discovered there
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    that the chimpanzees are very power hungry and wrote a book about it.
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    And at that time the focus in a lot of animal research
  • 1:02 - 1:04
    was on aggression and competition.
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    I painted a whole picture of the animal kingdom,
  • 1:06 - 1:08
    and humanity included,
  • 1:08 - 1:10
    was that deep down we are competitors,
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    we are aggressive,
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    we're all out for our own profit basically.
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    This is the launch of my book.
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    I'm not sure how well the chimpanzees read it,
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    but they surely seemed interested in the book.
  • 1:24 - 1:26
    Now in the process
  • 1:26 - 1:28
    of doing all this work on power and dominance
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    and aggression and so on,
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    I discovered that chimpanzees reconcile after fights.
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    And so what you see here is two males who have had a fight.
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    They ended up in a tree, and one of them holds out a hand to the other.
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    And about a second after I took the picture, they came together in the fork of the tree
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    and they kissed and embraced each other.
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    Now this is very interesting
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    because at the time everything was about competition and aggression,
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    and so it wouldn't make any sense.
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    The only thing that matters is that you win or that you lose.
  • 1:53 - 1:55
    But why would you reconcile after a fight?
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    That doesn't make any sense.
  • 1:57 - 2:00
    This is the way bonobos do it. Bonobos do everything with sex.
  • 2:00 - 2:02
    And so they also reconcile with sex.
  • 2:02 - 2:04
    But the principle is exactly the same.
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    The principle is that you have
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    a valuable relationship
  • 2:08 - 2:10
    that is damaged by conflict,
  • 2:10 - 2:12
    so you need to do something about it.
  • 2:12 - 2:14
    So my whole picture of the animal kingdom,
  • 2:14 - 2:16
    and including humans also,
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    started to change at that time.
  • 2:18 - 2:20
    So we have this image
  • 2:20 - 2:22
    in political science, economics, the humanities,
  • 2:22 - 2:24
    philosophy for that matter,
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    that man is a wolf to man.
  • 2:26 - 2:29
    And so deep down our nature's actually nasty.
  • 2:29 - 2:32
    I think it's a very unfair image for the wolf.
  • 2:32 - 2:34
    The wolf is, after all,
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    a very cooperative animal.
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    And that's why many of you have a dog at home,
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    which has all these characteristics also.
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    And it's really unfair to humanity,
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    because humanity is actually much more cooperative and empathic
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    than given credit for.
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    So I started getting interested in those issues
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    and studying that in other animals.
  • 2:52 - 2:54
    So these are the pillars of morality.
  • 2:54 - 2:58
    If you ask anyone, "What is morality based on?"
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    these are the two factors that always come out.
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    One is reciprocity,
  • 3:02 - 3:05
    and associated with it is a sense of justice and a sense of fairness.
  • 3:05 - 3:07
    And the other one is empathy and compassion.
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    And human morality is more than this,
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    but if you would remove these two pillars,
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    there would be not much remaining I think.
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    And so they're absolutely essential.
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    So let me give you a few examples here.
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    This is a very old video
    from the Yerkes Primate Center
  • 3:20 - 3:23
    where they train chimpanzees to cooperate.
  • 3:23 - 3:26
    ["1937"] So this is already
    about a hundred years ago
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    that we were doing experiments on cooperation.
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    What you have here is two young chimpanzees who have a box,
  • 3:32 - 3:35
    and the box is too heavy for one chimp to pull in.
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    And of course, there's food on the box.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    Otherwise they wouldn't be pulling so hard.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    And so they're bringing in the box.
  • 3:41 - 3:43
    And you can see that they're synchronized.
  • 3:43 - 3:46
    You can see that they work together, they pull at the same moment.
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    It's already a big advance over many other animals
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    who wouldn't be able to do that.
  • 3:51 - 3:53
    And now you're going to get a more interesting picture,
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    because now one of the two chimps has been fed.
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    So one of the two is not really interested
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    in the task anymore.
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    (Laughter)
  • 4:08 - 4:13
    (Laughter)
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    (Laughter)
  • 4:36 - 4:38
    Now look at what happens
    at the very end of this.
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    (Laughter)
  • 4:52 - 4:54
    He takes basically everything.
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    (Laughter)
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    So there are two interesting parts about this.
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    One is that the chimp on the right
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    has a full understanding he needs the partner --
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    so a full understanding of the need for cooperation.
  • 5:05 - 5:08
    The second one is that the partner is willing to work
  • 5:08 - 5:10
    even though he's not interested in the food.
  • 5:10 - 5:13
    Why would that be? Well that probably has to do with reciprocity.
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    There's actually a lot of evidence in primates and other animals
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    that they return favors.
  • 5:17 - 5:19
    So he will get a return favor
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    at some point in the future.
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    And so that's how this all operates.
  • 5:23 - 5:25
    We do the same task with elephants.
  • 5:25 - 5:28
    Now with elephants, it's very dangerous to work with elephants.
  • 5:28 - 5:30
    Another problem with elephants
  • 5:30 - 5:32
    is that you cannot make an apparatus
  • 5:32 - 5:34
    that is too heavy for a single elephant.
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    Now you can probably make it,
  • 5:36 - 5:38
    but it's going to be a pretty flimsy apparatus I think.
  • 5:38 - 5:40
    And so what we did in that case --
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    we do these studies in Thailand for Josh Plotnik --
  • 5:43 - 5:46
    is we have an apparatus around which there is a rope, a single rope.
  • 5:46 - 5:48
    And if you pull on this side of the rope,
  • 5:48 - 5:50
    the rope disappears on the other side.
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    So two elephants need to pick it up at exactly the same time and pull.
  • 5:53 - 5:55
    Otherwise nothing is going to happen
  • 5:55 - 5:57
    and the rope disappears.
  • 5:57 - 5:59
    And the first tape you're going to see
  • 5:59 - 6:01
    is two elephants who are released together
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    arrive at the apparatus.
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    The apparatus is on the left with food on it.
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    And so they come together, they arrive together,
  • 6:09 - 6:11
    they pick it up together and they pull together.
  • 6:11 - 6:14
    So it's actually fairly simple for them.
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    There they are.
  • 6:24 - 6:26
    And so that's how they bring it in.
  • 6:26 - 6:28
    But now we're going to make it more difficult.
  • 6:28 - 6:30
    Because the whole purpose of this experiment
  • 6:30 - 6:32
    is to see how well they understand cooperation.
  • 6:32 - 6:35
    Do they understand that as well as the chimps, for example?
  • 6:35 - 6:37
    And so what we do in the next step
  • 6:37 - 6:39
    is we release one elephant before the other,
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    and that elephant needs to be smart enough
  • 6:41 - 6:43
    to stay there and wait and not pull at the rope --
  • 6:43 - 6:46
    because if he pulls at the rope, it disappears and the whole test is over.
  • 6:46 - 6:48
    Now this elephant does something illegal
  • 6:48 - 6:50
    that we did not teach it.
  • 6:50 - 6:52
    But it shows the understanding that he has,
  • 6:52 - 6:55
    because he puts his big foot on the rope,
  • 6:55 - 6:57
    stands on the rope and waits there for the other,
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    and then the other is going to do all the work for him.
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    So it's what we call freeloading.
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    (Laughter)
  • 7:05 - 7:08
    But it shows the intelligence that the elephants have.
  • 7:08 - 7:11
    They develop several of these alternative techniques
  • 7:11 - 7:14
    that we did not approve of necessarily.
  • 7:14 - 7:19
    So the other elephant is now coming
  • 7:19 - 7:22
    and is going to pull it in.
  • 7:38 - 7:41
    Now look at the other. The other doesn't forget to eat, of course.
  • 7:41 - 7:45
    (Laughter)
  • 7:45 - 7:47
    This was the cooperation, reciprocity part.
  • 7:47 - 7:49
    Now something on empathy.
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    Empathy is my main topic at the moment of research.
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    And empathy has sort of two qualities.
  • 7:53 - 7:56
    One is the understanding part of it. This is just a regular definition:
  • 7:56 - 7:58
    the ability to understand and share the feelings of another.
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    And the emotional part.
  • 8:00 - 8:02
    And so empathy has basically two channels.
  • 8:02 - 8:04
    One is the body channel.
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    If you talk with a sad person,
  • 8:06 - 8:09
    you're going to adopt a sad expression and a sad posture,
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    and before you know it you feel sad.
  • 8:11 - 8:14
    And that's sort of the body channel of emotional empathy,
  • 8:14 - 8:16
    which many animals have.
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    Your average dog has that also.
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    That's actually why people keep mammals in the home
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    and not turtles or snakes or something like that
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    who don't have that kind of empathy.
  • 8:24 - 8:26
    And then there's a cognitive channel,
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    which is more that you can take the perspective of somebody else.
  • 8:28 - 8:30
    And that's more limited.
  • 8:30 - 8:32
    There's few animals -- I think elephants and apes can do that kind of thing --
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    but there are very few animals who can do that.
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    So synchronization,
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    which is part of that whole empathy mechanism
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    is a very old one in the animal kingdom.
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    And in humans, of course, we can study that
  • 8:43 - 8:45
    with yawn contagion.
  • 8:45 - 8:47
    Humans yawn when others yawn.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    And it's related to empathy.
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    It activates the same areas in the brain.
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    Also, we know that people who have a lot of yawn contagion
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    are highly empathic.
  • 8:55 - 8:57
    People who have problems with empathy, such as autistic children,
  • 8:57 - 8:59
    they don't have yawn contagion.
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    So it is connected.
  • 9:01 - 9:04
    And we study that in our chimpanzees by presenting them with an animated head.
  • 9:04 - 9:06
    So that's what you see on the upper-left,
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    an animated head that yawns.
  • 9:08 - 9:10
    And there's a chimpanzee watching,
  • 9:10 - 9:13
    an actual real chimpanzee watching a computer screen
  • 9:13 - 9:16
    on which we play these animations.
  • 9:20 - 9:22
    (Laughter)
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    So yawn contagion
  • 9:24 - 9:26
    that you're probably all familiar with --
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    and maybe you're going to start yawning soon now --
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    is something that we share with other animals.
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    And that's related to that whole body channel of synchronization
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    that underlies empathy
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    and that is universal in the mammals basically.
  • 9:40 - 9:43
    Now we also study more complex expressions. This is consolation.
  • 9:43 - 9:46
    This is a male chimpanzee who has lost a fight and he's screaming,
  • 9:46 - 9:48
    and a juvenile comes over and puts an arm around him
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    and calms him down.
  • 9:50 - 9:53
    That's consolation. It's very similar to human consolation.
  • 9:53 - 9:56
    And consolation behavior,
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    it's empathy driven.
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    Actually the way to study empathy in human children
  • 10:01 - 10:03
    is to instruct a family member to act distressed,
  • 10:03 - 10:05
    and then they see what young children do.
  • 10:05 - 10:07
    And so it is related to empathy,
  • 10:07 - 10:10
    and that's the kind of expressions we look at.
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    We also recently published an experiment you may have heard about.
  • 10:13 - 10:16
    It's on altruism and chimpanzees
  • 10:16 - 10:18
    where the question is, do chimpanzees care
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    about the welfare of somebody else?
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    And for decades it had been assumed
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    that only humans can do that,
  • 10:24 - 10:27
    that only humans worry about the welfare of somebody else.
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    Now we did a very simple experiment.
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    We do that on chimpanzees that live in Lawrenceville,
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    in the field station of Yerkes.
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    And so that's how they live.
  • 10:36 - 10:39
    And we call them into a room and do experiments with them.
  • 10:39 - 10:41
    In this case, we put two chimpanzees side-by-side.
  • 10:41 - 10:44
    and one has a bucket full of tokens, and the tokens have different meanings.
  • 10:44 - 10:47
    One kind of token feeds only the partner who chooses,
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    the other one feeds both of them.
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    So this is a study we did with Vicky Horner.
  • 10:53 - 10:55
    And here you have the two color tokens.
  • 10:55 - 10:57
    So they have a whole bucket full of them.
  • 10:57 - 11:00
    And they have to pick one of the two colors.
  • 11:00 - 11:03
    You will see how that goes.
  • 11:03 - 11:06
    So if this chimp makes the selfish choice,
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    which is the red token in this case,
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    he needs to give it to us.
  • 11:11 - 11:14
    So we pick it up, we put it on a table where there's two food rewards,
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    but in this case only the one on the right gets food.
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    The one on the left walks away because she knows already.
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    that this is not a good test for her.
  • 11:22 - 11:24
    Then the next one is the pro-social token.
  • 11:24 - 11:27
    So the one who makes the choices -- that's the interesting part here --
  • 11:27 - 11:29
    for the one who makes the choices,
  • 11:29 - 11:31
    it doesn't really matter.
  • 11:31 - 11:34
    So she gives us now a pro-social token and both chimps get fed.
  • 11:34 - 11:37
    So the one who makes the choices always gets a reward.
  • 11:37 - 11:39
    So it doesn't matter whatsoever.
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    And she should actually be choosing blindly.
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    But what we find
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    is that they prefer the pro-social token.
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    So this is the 50 percent line that's the random expectation.
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    And especially if the partner draws attention to itself, they choose more.
  • 11:51 - 11:54
    And if the partner puts pressure on them --
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    so if the partner starts spitting water and intimidating them --
  • 11:57 - 12:00
    then the choices go down.
  • 12:00 - 12:02
    It's as if they're saying,
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    "If you're not behaving, I'm not going to be pro-social today."
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    And this is what happens without a partner,
  • 12:06 - 12:08
    when there's no partner sitting there.
  • 12:08 - 12:10
    And so we found that the chimpanzees do care
  • 12:10 - 12:12
    about the well-being of somebody else --
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    especially, these are other members of their own group.
  • 12:15 - 12:18
    So the final experiment that I want to mention to you
  • 12:18 - 12:20
    is our fairness study.
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    And so this became a very famous study.
  • 12:23 - 12:25
    And there's now many more,
  • 12:25 - 12:27
    because after we did this about 10 years ago,
  • 12:27 - 12:29
    it became very well known.
  • 12:29 - 12:31
    And we did that originally with capuchin monkeys.
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    And I'm going to show you the first experiment that we did.
  • 12:34 - 12:37
    It has now been done with dogs and with birds
  • 12:37 - 12:39
    and with chimpanzees.
  • 12:39 - 12:43
    But with Sarah Brosnan we started out with capuchin monkeys.
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    So what we did
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    is we put two capuchin monkeys side-by-side.
  • 12:47 - 12:49
    Again, these animals, they live in a group, they know each other.
  • 12:49 - 12:52
    We take them out of the group, put them in a test chamber.
  • 12:52 - 12:54
    And there's a very simple task
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    that they need to do.
  • 12:56 - 12:59
    And if you give both of them cucumber for the task,
  • 12:59 - 13:01
    the two monkeys side-by-side,
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    they're perfectly willing to do this 25 times in a row.
  • 13:03 - 13:07
    So cucumber, even though it's only really water in my opinion,
  • 13:07 - 13:10
    but cucumber is perfectly fine for them.
  • 13:10 - 13:13
    Now if you give the partner grapes --
  • 13:13 - 13:15
    the food preferences of my capuchin monkeys
  • 13:15 - 13:18
    correspond exactly with the prices in the supermarket --
  • 13:18 - 13:21
    and so if you give them grapes -- it's a far better food --
  • 13:21 - 13:24
    then you create inequity between them.
  • 13:24 - 13:26
    So that's the experiment we did.
  • 13:26 - 13:29
    Recently we videotaped it with new monkeys who'd never done the task,
  • 13:29 - 13:31
    thinking that maybe they would have a stronger reaction,
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    and that turned out to be right.
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    The one on the left is the monkey who gets cucumber.
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    The one on the right is the one who gets grapes.
  • 13:38 - 13:40
    The one who gets cucumber,
  • 13:40 - 13:42
    note that the first piece of cucumber is perfectly fine.
  • 13:42 - 13:45
    The first piece she eats.
  • 13:45 - 13:48
    Then she sees the other one getting grape, and you will see what happens.
  • 13:48 - 13:51
    So she gives a rock to us. That's the task.
  • 13:51 - 13:54
    And we give her a piece of cucumber and she eats it.
  • 13:54 - 13:57
    The other one needs to give a rock to us.
  • 13:57 - 14:00
    And that's what she does.
  • 14:00 - 14:03
    And she gets a grape and she eats it.
  • 14:03 - 14:05
    The other one sees that.
  • 14:05 - 14:07
    She gives a rock to us now,
  • 14:07 - 14:10
    gets, again, cucumber.
  • 14:12 - 14:27
    (Laughter)
  • 14:27 - 14:30
    She tests a rock now against the wall.
  • 14:30 - 14:32
    She needs to give it to us.
  • 14:32 - 14:35
    And she gets cucumber again.
  • 14:37 - 14:41
    (Laughter)
  • 14:43 - 14:47
    So this is basically the Wall Street protest that you see here.
  • 14:47 - 14:50
    (Laughter)
  • 14:50 - 14:53
    (Applause)
  • 14:53 - 14:55
    Let me tell you --
  • 14:55 - 14:57
    I still have two minutes left, let me tell you a funny story about this.
  • 14:57 - 14:59
    This study became very famous
  • 14:59 - 15:01
    and we got a lot of comments,
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    especially anthropologists, economists,
  • 15:03 - 15:05
    philosophers.
  • 15:05 - 15:07
    They didn't like this at all.
  • 15:07 - 15:10
    Because they had decided in their minds, I believe,
  • 15:10 - 15:12
    that fairness is a very complex issue
  • 15:12 - 15:14
    and that animals cannot have it.
  • 15:14 - 15:16
    And so one philosopher even wrote us
  • 15:16 - 15:19
    that it was impossible that monkeys had a sense of fairness
  • 15:19 - 15:22
    because fairness was invented during the French Revolution.
  • 15:22 - 15:24
    (Laughter)
  • 15:24 - 15:27
    Now another one wrote a whole chapter
  • 15:27 - 15:31
    saying that he would believe it had something to do with fairness
  • 15:31 - 15:33
    if the one who got grapes would refuse the grapes.
  • 15:33 - 15:35
    Now the funny thing is that Sarah Brosnan,
  • 15:35 - 15:37
    who's been doing this with chimpanzees,
  • 15:37 - 15:39
    had a couple of combinations of chimpanzees
  • 15:39 - 15:42
    where, indeed, the one who would get the grape would refuse the grape
  • 15:42 - 15:44
    until the other guy also got a grape.
  • 15:44 - 15:47
    So we're getting very close to the human sense of fairness.
  • 15:47 - 15:51
    And I think philosophers need to rethink their philosophy for awhile.
  • 15:51 - 15:53
    So let me summarize.
  • 15:53 - 15:55
    I believe there's an evolved morality.
  • 15:55 - 15:57
    I think morality is much more than what I've been talking about,
  • 15:57 - 16:00
    but it would be impossible without these ingredients
  • 16:00 - 16:02
    that we find in other primates,
  • 16:02 - 16:04
    which are empathy and consolation,
  • 16:04 - 16:07
    pro-social tendencies and reciprocity and a sense of fairness.
  • 16:07 - 16:10
    And so we work on these particular issues
  • 16:10 - 16:13
    to see if we can create a morality from the bottom up, so to speak,
  • 16:13 - 16:15
    without necessarily God and religion involved,
  • 16:15 - 16:18
    and to see how we can get to an evolved morality.
  • 16:18 - 16:21
    And I thank you for your attention.
  • 16:21 - 16:30
    (Applause)
Title:
Moral behavior in animals
Speaker:
Frans de Waal
Description:

Empathy, cooperation, fairness and reciprocity -- caring about the well-being of others seems like a very human trait. But Frans de Waal shares some surprising videos of behavioral tests, on primates and other mammals, that show how many of these moral traits all of us share.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
16:31
  • At 4:23, a subtitle has just been added that contains the on-screen text as displayed.

  • The English transcript was updated on 11/21/2016. At 05:36, "flimsy" was changed to "clumsy."

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions