Return to Video

Moral behavior in animals

  • 0:01 - 0:02
    I was born in Den Bosch,
  • 0:02 - 0:06
    where the painter Hieronymus Bosch
    named himself after.
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    And I've always been very fond
    of this painter
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    who lived and worked in the 15th century.
  • 0:11 - 0:13
    And what is interesting about him
    in relation to morality
  • 0:13 - 0:16
    is that he lived at a time
    where religion's influence was waning,
  • 0:16 - 0:18
    and he was sort of wondering, I think,
  • 0:18 - 0:21
    what would happen with society
    if there was no religion
  • 0:21 - 0:22
    or if there was less religion.
  • 0:22 - 0:26
    And so he painted this famous painting,
    "The Garden of Earthly Delights,"
  • 0:26 - 0:30
    which some have interpreted
    as being humanity before the Fall,
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    or being humanity without any Fall at all.
  • 0:33 - 0:34
    And so it makes you wonder,
  • 0:34 - 0:38
    what would happen if we hadn't tasted
    the fruit of knowledge, so to speak,
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    and what kind of morality would we have.
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    Much later, as a student,
    I went to a very different garden,
  • 0:44 - 0:49
    a zoological garden in Arnhem
    where we keep chimpanzees.
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    This is me at an early age
    with a baby chimpanzee.
  • 0:52 - 0:55
    (Laughter)
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    And I discovered there
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    that the chimpanzees are very power-hungry
    and wrote a book about it.
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    And at that time the focus
    in a lot of animal research
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    was on aggression and competition.
  • 1:05 - 1:07
    I painted a whole picture
    of the animal kingdom
  • 1:07 - 1:12
    and humanity included, was that deep down
    we are competitors, we are aggressive,
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    we are all out
    for our own profit, basically.
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    This is the launch of my book.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    I'm not sure how well
    the chimpanzees read it,
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    but they surely seemed
    interested in the book.
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    (Laughter)
  • 1:25 - 1:28
    Now in the process of doing all this work
  • 1:28 - 1:31
    on power and dominance
    and aggression and so on,
  • 1:31 - 1:34
    I discovered that chimpanzees
    reconcile after fights.
  • 1:34 - 1:37
    And so what you see here
    is two males who have had a fight.
  • 1:37 - 1:40
    They ended up in a tree, and one of them
    holds out a hand to the other.
  • 1:40 - 1:42
    And about a second
    after I took the picture,
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    they came together in the fork of the tree
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    and kissed and embraced each other.
  • 1:46 - 1:47
    And this is very interesting
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    because at the time, everything
    was about competition and aggression,
  • 1:51 - 1:52
    so it wouldn't make any sense.
  • 1:52 - 1:55
    The only thing that matters
    is that you win or you lose.
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    But why reconcile after a fight?
    That doesn't make any sense.
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    This is the way bonobos do it.
    Bonobos do everything with sex.
  • 2:01 - 2:03
    And so they also reconcile with sex.
  • 2:03 - 2:04
    But the principle is exactly the same.
  • 2:05 - 2:08
    The principle is that you have
    a valuable relationship
  • 2:09 - 2:13
    that is damaged by conflict,
    so you need to do something about it.
  • 2:13 - 2:17
    So my whole picture of the animal kingdom,
    and including humans also,
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    started to change at that time.
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    So we have this image in political
    science, economics, the humanities,
  • 2:23 - 2:27
    the philosophy for that matter,
    that man is a wolf to man.
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    And so deep down,
    our nature is actually nasty.
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    I think it's a very unfair
    image for the wolf.
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    The wolf is, after all,
    a very cooperative animal.
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    And that's why many of you
    have a dog at home,
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    which has all these characteristics also.
  • 2:41 - 2:43
    And it's really unfair to humanity,
  • 2:43 - 2:46
    because humanity is actually
    much more cooperative and empathic
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    than given credit for.
  • 2:48 - 2:50
    So I started getting
    interested in those issues
  • 2:50 - 2:52
    and studying that in other animals.
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    So these are the pillars of morality.
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    If you ask anyone,
    "What is morality based on?"
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    these are the two factors
    that always come out.
  • 3:01 - 3:02
    One is reciprocity,
  • 3:02 - 3:06
    and associated with it is a sense
    of justice and a sense of fairness.
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    And the other one is empathy
    and compassion.
  • 3:08 - 3:12
    And human morality is more than this,
    but if you would remove these two pillars,
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    there would be not much
    remaining, I think.
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    So they're absolutely essential.
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    So let me give you a few examples here.
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    This is a very old video
    from the Yerkes Primate Center,
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    where they trained
    chimpanzees to cooperate.
  • 3:24 - 3:26
    So this is already
    about a hundred years ago
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    that we were doing
    experiments on cooperation.
  • 3:29 - 3:33
    What you have here is two
    young chimpanzees who have a box,
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    and the box is too heavy
    for one chimp to pull in.
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    And of course, there's food on the box.
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    Otherwise they wouldn't
    be pulling so hard.
  • 3:40 - 3:42
    And so they're bringing in the box.
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    And you can see that they're synchronized.
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    You can see that they work together,
    they pull at the same moment.
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    It's already a big advance
    over many other animals
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    who wouldn't be able to do that.
  • 3:52 - 3:54
    Now you're going to get
    a more interesting picture,
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    because now one
    of the two chimps has been fed.
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    So one of the two is not really interested
    in the task anymore.
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    (Laughter)
  • 4:09 - 4:14
    (Laughter)
  • 4:20 - 4:23
    (Laughter)
  • 4:23 - 4:28
    [- and sometimes appears to convey
    its wishes and meanings by gestures.]
  • 4:36 - 4:38
    Now look at what happens
    at the very end of this.
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    (Laughter)
  • 4:53 - 4:54
    He takes basically everything.
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    (Laughter)
  • 4:57 - 4:59
    There are two interesting
    parts about this.
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    One is that the chimp on the right
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    has a full understanding
    he needs the partner --
  • 5:03 - 5:06
    so a full understanding
    of the need for cooperation.
  • 5:06 - 5:08
    The second one is that the partner
    is willing to work
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    even though he's not
    interested in the food.
  • 5:11 - 5:12
    Why would that be?
  • 5:12 - 5:14
    Well, that probably
    has to do with reciprocity.
  • 5:14 - 5:17
    There's actually a lot of evidence
    in primates and other animals
  • 5:17 - 5:19
    that they return favors.
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    He will get a return favor
    at some point in the future.
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    And so that's how this all operates.
  • 5:24 - 5:26
    We do the same task with elephants.
  • 5:26 - 5:29
    Now, it's very dangerous
    to work with elephants.
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    Another problem with elephants
    is that you cannot make an apparatus
  • 5:32 - 5:34
    that is too heavy for a single elephant.
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    Now you can probably make it,
  • 5:36 - 5:39
    but it's going to be a pretty
    clumsy apparatus, I think.
  • 5:39 - 5:40
    And so what we did in that case --
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    we do these studies in Thailand
    for Josh Plotnik --
  • 5:43 - 5:47
    is we have an apparatus around which
    there is a rope, a single rope.
  • 5:47 - 5:51
    And if you pull on this side of the rope,
    the rope disappears on the other side.
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    So two elephants need to pick it up
    at exactly the same time, and pull.
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    Otherwise nothing is going to happen
    and the rope disappears.
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    The first tape you're going to see
  • 6:00 - 6:04
    is two elephants who are released together
    arrive at the apparatus.
  • 6:04 - 6:06
    The apparatus is on the left,
    with food on it.
  • 6:07 - 6:10
    And so they come together,
    they arrive together,
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    they pick it up together,
    and they pull together.
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    So it's actually fairly simple for them.
  • 6:16 - 6:17
    There they are.
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    So that's how they bring it in.
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    But now we're going to make it
    more difficult.
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    Because the purpose of this experiment
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    is to see how well
    they understand cooperation.
  • 6:33 - 6:36
    Do they understand that as well
    as the chimps, for example?
  • 6:36 - 6:39
    What we do in the next step is we release
    one elephant before the other
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    and that elephant needs to be smart enough
  • 6:41 - 6:44
    to stay there and wait
    and not pull at the rope --
  • 6:44 - 6:47
    because if he pulls at the rope,
    it disappears and the whole test is over.
  • 6:47 - 6:51
    Now this elephant does something illegal
    that we did not teach it.
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    But it shows the understanding he has,
  • 6:53 - 6:55
    because he puts his big foot on the rope,
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    stands on the rope
    and waits there for the other,
  • 6:58 - 7:00
    and then the other is going
    to do all the work for him.
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    So it's what we call freeloading.
  • 7:03 - 7:06
    (Laughter)
  • 7:06 - 7:09
    But it shows the intelligence
    that the elephants have.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    They developed several
    of these alternative techniques
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    that we did not approve of, necessarily.
  • 7:13 - 7:15
    (Laughter)
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    So the other elephant is now coming ...
  • 7:20 - 7:21
    and is going to pull it in.
  • 7:39 - 7:42
    Now look at the other;
    it doesn't forget to eat, of course.
  • 7:42 - 7:45
    (Laughter)
  • 7:46 - 7:48
    This was the cooperation
    and reciprocity part.
  • 7:48 - 7:49
    Now something on empathy.
  • 7:49 - 7:52
    Empathy is my main topic
    at the moment, of research.
  • 7:52 - 7:53
    And empathy has two qualities:
  • 7:53 - 7:55
    One is the understanding part of it.
  • 7:55 - 7:57
    This is just a regular definition:
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    the ability to understand and share
    the feelings of another.
  • 8:00 - 8:01
    And the emotional part.
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    Empathy has basically two channels:
    One is the body channel,
  • 8:04 - 8:06
    If you talk with a sad person,
  • 8:06 - 8:09
    you're going to adopt
    a sad expression and a sad posture,
  • 8:10 - 8:11
    and before you know it, you feel sad.
  • 8:11 - 8:16
    And that's sort of the body channel
    of emotional empathy,
  • 8:16 - 8:17
    which many animals have.
  • 8:17 - 8:18
    Your average dog has that also.
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    That's why people keep mammals in the home
  • 8:20 - 8:23
    and not turtles or snakes
    or something like that,
  • 8:23 - 8:25
    who don't have that kind of empathy.
  • 8:25 - 8:26
    And then there's a cognitive channel,
  • 8:26 - 8:29
    which is more that you can take
    the perspective of somebody else.
  • 8:29 - 8:31
    And that's more limited.
  • 8:31 - 8:35
    Very few animals, I think elephants
    and apes, can do that kind of thing.
  • 8:37 - 8:38
    So synchronization,
  • 8:38 - 8:40
    which is part of that whole
    empathy mechanism,
  • 8:40 - 8:42
    is a very old one in the animal kingdom.
  • 8:42 - 8:46
    In humans, of course,
    we can study that with yawn contagion.
  • 8:46 - 8:47
    Humans yawn when others yawn.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    And it's related to empathy.
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    It activates the same areas in the brain.
  • 8:51 - 8:54
    Also, we know that people
    who have a lot of yawn contagion
  • 8:54 - 8:55
    are highly empathic.
  • 8:55 - 8:59
    People who have problems with empathy,
    such as autistic children,
  • 8:59 - 9:00
    they don't have yawn contagion.
  • 9:00 - 9:01
    So it is connected.
  • 9:01 - 9:05
    And we study that in our chimpanzees
    by presenting them with an animated head.
  • 9:05 - 9:09
    So that's what you see on the upper-left,
    an animated head that yawns.
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    And there's a chimpanzee watching,
  • 9:11 - 9:13
    an actual real chimpanzee
    watching a computer screen
  • 9:13 - 9:15
    on which we play these animations.
  • 9:21 - 9:23
    (Laughter)
  • 9:23 - 9:26
    So yawn contagion
    that you're probably all familiar with --
  • 9:26 - 9:29
    and maybe you're going
    to start yawning soon now --
  • 9:29 - 9:32
    is something that we share
    with other animals.
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    And that's related to that whole
    body channel of synchronization
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    that underlies empathy,
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    and that is universal
    in the mammals, basically.
  • 9:40 - 9:43
    We also study more complex expressions --
    This is consolation.
  • 9:43 - 9:47
    This is a male chimpanzee
    who has lost a fight and he's screaming,
  • 9:47 - 9:49
    and a juvenile comes over
    and puts an arm around him
  • 9:49 - 9:50
    and calms him down.
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    That's consolation.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    It's very similar to human consolation.
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    And consolation behavior --
  • 9:56 - 9:57
    (Laughter)
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    it's empathy driven.
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    Actually, the way to study
    empathy in human children
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    is to instruct a family member
    to act distressed,
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    and then to see what young children do.
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    And so it is related to empathy,
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    and that's the kind
    of expressions we look at.
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    We also recently published an experiment
    you may have heard about.
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    It's on altruism and chimpanzees,
  • 10:15 - 10:20
    where the question is: Do chimpanzees care
    about the welfare of somebody else?
  • 10:20 - 10:24
    And for decades it had been assumed
    that only humans can do that,
  • 10:24 - 10:28
    that only humans worry
    about the welfare of somebody else.
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    Now we did a very simple experiment.
  • 10:30 - 10:33
    We do that on chimpanzees
    that live in Lawrenceville,
  • 10:33 - 10:35
    in the field station of Yerkes.
  • 10:35 - 10:36
    And so that's how they live.
  • 10:36 - 10:39
    And we call them into a room
    and do experiments with them.
  • 10:39 - 10:41
    In this case, we put
    two chimpanzees side-by-side,
  • 10:41 - 10:45
    and one has a bucket full of tokens,
    and the tokens have different meanings.
  • 10:45 - 10:48
    One kind of token feeds
    only the partner who chooses,
  • 10:48 - 10:50
    the other one feeds both of them.
  • 10:50 - 10:52
    So this is a study we did
    with Vicki Horner.
  • 10:54 - 10:56
    And here, you have the two color tokens.
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    So they have a whole bucket full of them.
  • 10:58 - 11:01
    And they have to pick
    one of the two colors.
  • 11:01 - 11:03
    You will see how that goes.
  • 11:04 - 11:06
    So if this chimp makes the selfish choice,
  • 11:07 - 11:10
    which is the red token in this case,
  • 11:10 - 11:11
    he needs to give it to us,
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    we pick it up, we put it on a table
    where there's two food rewards,
  • 11:15 - 11:17
    but in this case, only the one
    on the right gets food.
  • 11:17 - 11:20
    The one on the left walks away
    because she knows already
  • 11:20 - 11:22
    that this is not a good test for her.
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    Then the next one is the pro-social token.
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    So the one who makes the choices --
    that's the interesting part here --
  • 11:28 - 11:31
    for the one who makes the choices,
    it doesn't really matter.
  • 11:31 - 11:34
    So she gives us now a pro-social
    token and both chimps get fed.
  • 11:34 - 11:37
    So the one who makes the choices
    always gets a reward.
  • 11:37 - 11:39
    So it doesn't matter whatsoever.
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    And she should actually
    be choosing blindly.
  • 11:42 - 11:45
    But what we find is that they prefer
    the pro-social token.
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    So this is the 50 percent line,
    that's the random expectation.
  • 11:48 - 11:52
    And especially if the partner draws
    attention to itself, they choose more.
  • 11:52 - 11:54
    And if the partner
    puts pressure on them --
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    so if the partner starts spitting water
    and intimidating them --
  • 11:57 - 11:59
    then the choices go down.
  • 11:59 - 12:00
    (Laughter)
  • 12:00 - 12:02
    It's as if they're saying,
  • 12:02 - 12:05
    "If you're not behaving,
    I'm not going to be pro-social today."
  • 12:05 - 12:07
    And this is what happens
    without a partner,
  • 12:07 - 12:09
    when there's no partner sitting there.
  • 12:09 - 12:13
    So we found that the chimpanzees do care
    about the well-being of somebody else --
  • 12:13 - 12:16
    especially, these are other members
    of their own group.
  • 12:16 - 12:19
    So the final experiment
    that I want to mention to you
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    is our fairness study.
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    And so this became a very famous study.
  • 12:24 - 12:25
    And there are now many more,
  • 12:25 - 12:27
    because after we did this
    about 10 years ago,
  • 12:27 - 12:29
    it became very well-known.
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    And we did that originally
    with capuchin monkeys.
  • 12:32 - 12:35
    And I'm going to show you
    the first experiment that we did.
  • 12:35 - 12:37
    It has now been done
    with dogs and with birds
  • 12:37 - 12:39
    and with chimpanzees.
  • 12:40 - 12:43
    But with Sarah Brosnan,
    we started out with capuchin monkeys.
  • 12:44 - 12:48
    So what we did is we put
    two capuchin monkeys side-by-side.
  • 12:48 - 12:50
    Again, these animals, live in a group,
    they know each other.
  • 12:50 - 12:53
    We take them out of the group,
    put them in a test chamber.
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    And there's a very simple task
    that they need to do.
  • 12:56 - 13:00
    And if you give both of them
    cucumber for the task,
  • 13:00 - 13:01
    the two monkeys side-by-side,
  • 13:01 - 13:04
    they're perfectly willing
    to do this 25 times in a row.
  • 13:04 - 13:08
    So cucumber, even though
    it's only really water in my opinion,
  • 13:08 - 13:11
    but cucumber is perfectly fine for them.
  • 13:11 - 13:13
    Now if you give the partner grapes --
  • 13:13 - 13:15
    the food preferences
    of my capuchin monkeys
  • 13:15 - 13:18
    correspond exactly with the prices
    in the supermarket --
  • 13:18 - 13:22
    and so if you give them grapes --
    it's a far better food --
  • 13:22 - 13:24
    then you create inequity between them.
  • 13:25 - 13:27
    So that's the experiment we did.
  • 13:27 - 13:29
    Recently, we videotaped it
    with new monkeys
  • 13:29 - 13:30
    who'd never done the task,
  • 13:30 - 13:33
    thinking that maybe they would have
    a stronger reaction,
  • 13:33 - 13:34
    and that turned out to be right.
  • 13:34 - 13:37
    The one on the left is the monkey
    who gets cucumber.
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    The one on the right
    is the one who gets grapes.
  • 13:39 - 13:40
    The one who gets cucumber --
  • 13:40 - 13:43
    note that the first piece
    of cucumber is perfectly fine.
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    The first piece she eats.
  • 13:45 - 13:49
    Then she sees the other one getting grape,
    and you will see what happens.
  • 13:49 - 13:52
    So she gives a rock to us.
    That's the task.
  • 13:52 - 13:55
    And we give her a piece
    of cucumber and she eats it.
  • 13:55 - 13:57
    The other one needs to give a rock to us.
  • 13:58 - 14:00
    And that's what she does.
  • 14:01 - 14:02
    And she gets a grape ...
  • 14:04 - 14:05
    and eats it.
  • 14:05 - 14:06
    The other one sees that.
  • 14:06 - 14:07
    She gives a rock to us now,
  • 14:07 - 14:09
    gets, again, cucumber.
  • 14:12 - 14:19
    (Laughter)
  • 14:26 - 14:28
    (Laughter ends)
  • 14:28 - 14:31
    She tests a rock now against the wall.
  • 14:31 - 14:32
    She needs to give it to us.
  • 14:33 - 14:35
    And she gets cucumber again.
  • 14:37 - 14:44
    (Laughter)
  • 14:44 - 14:48
    So this is basically
    the Wall Street protest that you see here.
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    (Laughter)
  • 14:50 - 14:54
    (Applause)
  • 14:54 - 14:56
    I still have two minutes left --
  • 14:56 - 14:58
    let me tell you a funny story about this.
  • 14:58 - 15:02
    This study became very famous
    and we got a lot of comments,
  • 15:02 - 15:06
    especially anthropologists,
    economists, philosophers.
  • 15:06 - 15:07
    They didn't like this at all.
  • 15:07 - 15:10
    Because they had decided
    in their minds, I believe,
  • 15:10 - 15:14
    that fairness is a very complex issue,
    and that animals cannot have it.
  • 15:14 - 15:17
    And so one philosopher even wrote us
  • 15:17 - 15:19
    that it was impossible that monkeys
    had a sense of fairness
  • 15:19 - 15:22
    because fairness was invented
    during the French Revolution.
  • 15:22 - 15:25
    (Laughter)
  • 15:25 - 15:27
    And another one wrote a whole chapter
  • 15:27 - 15:31
    saying that he would believe
    it had something to do with fairness,
  • 15:31 - 15:33
    if the one who got grapes
    would refuse the grapes.
  • 15:33 - 15:37
    Now the funny thing is that Sarah Brosnan,
    who's been doing this with chimpanzees,
  • 15:37 - 15:39
    had a couple of combinations
    of chimpanzees
  • 15:39 - 15:42
    where, indeed, the one
    who would get the grape
  • 15:42 - 15:45
    would refuse the grape
    until the other guy also got a grape.
  • 15:45 - 15:48
    So we're getting very close
    to the human sense of fairness.
  • 15:48 - 15:51
    And I think philosophers need
    to rethink their philosophy for a while.
  • 15:52 - 15:53
    So let me summarize.
  • 15:54 - 15:55
    I believe there's an evolved morality.
  • 15:55 - 15:59
    I think morality is much more
    than what I've been talking about,
  • 15:59 - 16:01
    but it would be impossible
    without these ingredients
  • 16:01 - 16:03
    that we find in other primates,
  • 16:03 - 16:05
    which are empathy and consolation,
  • 16:05 - 16:08
    pro-social tendencies and reciprocity
    and a sense of fairness.
  • 16:08 - 16:10
    And so we work on these particular issues
  • 16:10 - 16:14
    to see if we can create a morality
    from the bottom up, so to speak,
  • 16:14 - 16:16
    without necessarily
    god and religion involved,
  • 16:16 - 16:19
    and to see how we can get
    to an evolved morality.
  • 16:19 - 16:21
    And I thank you for your attention.
  • 16:21 - 16:28
    (Applause)
Title:
Moral behavior in animals
Speaker:
Frans de Waal
Description:

Empathy, cooperation, fairness and reciprocity -- caring about the well-being of others seems like a very human trait. But Frans de Waal shares some surprising videos of behavioral tests, on primates and other mammals, that show how many of these moral traits all of us share.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
16:31
  • At 4:23, a subtitle has just been added that contains the on-screen text as displayed.

  • The English transcript was updated on 11/21/2016. At 05:36, "flimsy" was changed to "clumsy."

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions