Return to Video

A new way to diagnose autism | Ami Klin | TEDxPeachtree

  • 0:02 - 0:04
    Host: We're going to move along
    with our content,
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    providing it for you as we go on,
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    our next presenter today
    is doctor Ami Klin.
  • 0:08 - 0:13
    He refers to himself as a walking
    laboratory of social engagement.
  • 0:13 - 0:16
    And I'll tell you, the man just
    minding his own business at Yale,
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    and Home Depot co-founder,
    Bernie Marcus,
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    went up there and persuaded him
    to come down south,
  • 0:23 - 0:27
    where he is now the new director
    of the Marcus Autism Center.
  • 0:27 - 0:29
    He is here today to share with us
    some viewpoints
  • 0:29 - 0:33
    on how early targeted
    social engagement with autistic children
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    can be an investment
    in their future productivity.
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    I'd like you to join me in welcoming him,
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    to share an idea worth sharing -
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    autism, disruptions is early human
    social adaptation mechanisms.
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    Welcoming doctor Ami Klin.
  • 0:46 - 0:51
    (Applause)
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    Ami Klin: For me,
    autism has been a personal journey.
  • 1:06 - 1:07
    As I was introduced,
  • 1:08 - 1:12
    I always wanted to become
    a walking laboratory of social engagement:
  • 1:14 - 1:18
    to resonate other people's feelings,
    thoughts, intentions, motivations,
  • 1:18 - 1:20
    in the act of being with them.
  • 1:21 - 1:26
    As a scientist, I always wanted
    to measure that resonance,
  • 1:27 - 1:30
    that sense of the other
    that happens so quickly,
  • 1:30 - 1:31
    in the blink of an eye.
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    We intuit other people's feelings;
  • 1:34 - 1:38
    we know the meaning of their actions
    even before they happen.
  • 1:38 - 1:40
    We're always in this stance
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    of being the object
    of somebody else's subjectivity.
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    We do that all the time.
    We just can't shake it off.
  • 1:46 - 1:50
    It's so important that the very tools
    we use to understand ourselves,
  • 1:50 - 1:52
    to understand the world around us,
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    are shaped by that stance.
  • 1:55 - 1:57
    We are social to the core.
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    So my journey in autism really started
  • 2:00 - 2:04
    when I lived in a residential unit
    for adults with autism.
  • 2:04 - 2:08
    Most of those individuals
    had spent most of their lives
  • 2:08 - 2:09
    in long-stay hospitals.
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    This is a long time ago.
  • 2:11 - 2:15
    And for them, autism was devastating.
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    They had profound
    intellectual disabilities.
  • 2:19 - 2:20
    They didn't talk.
  • 2:21 - 2:22
    But most of all,
  • 2:23 - 2:28
    they were extraordinarily isolated
    from the world around them,
  • 2:28 - 2:29
    from their environment
  • 2:29 - 2:31
    and from the people.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    In fact, at the time,
    if you walked into a school
  • 2:35 - 2:36
    for individuals with autism,
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    you'd hear a lot of noise,
  • 2:39 - 2:43
    plenty of commotion, actions,
    people doing things.
  • 2:44 - 2:47
    But they're always doing
    things by themselves.
  • 2:48 - 2:51
    So they may be looking
    at a light in the ceiling,
  • 2:52 - 2:55
    or they may be isolated in the corner,
  • 2:55 - 2:59
    or they might be engaged
    in these repetitive movements,
  • 2:59 - 3:02
    in self-stimulatory movements
    that led them nowhere.
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    Extremely, extremely isolated.
  • 3:07 - 3:13
    Well, now we know that autism
    is this disruption,
  • 3:13 - 3:16
    the disruption of this resonance
    that I am telling you about.
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    These are survival skills.
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    These are survival skills
    that we inherited
  • 3:21 - 3:24
    over many, many hundreds
    of thousands of years of evolution.
  • 3:25 - 3:30
    You see, babies are born
    in a state of utter fragility.
  • 3:30 - 3:33
    Without the caregiver,
    they wouldn't survive,
  • 3:33 - 3:35
    so it stands to reason
    that nature would endow them
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    with these mechanisms of survival.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    They orient to the caregiver.
  • 3:41 - 3:44
    From the first days and weeks of life,
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    babies prefer to hear human sounds,
  • 3:47 - 3:49
    rather than just sounds
    in the environment.
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    They prefer to look at people
    rather than at things,
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    and even as they're looking at people,
    they look at people's eyes,
  • 3:56 - 4:00
    because the eye is the window
    to the other person's experiences,
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    so much so that they even prefer
    to look at people
  • 4:03 - 4:06
    who are looking at them
    rather than people who are looking away.
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    Well, they orient to the caregiver.
  • 4:10 - 4:11
    The caregiver seeks the baby.
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    And it's out of this mutually
    reinforcing choreography
  • 4:15 - 4:19
    that a lot that is of importance
    to the emergence of mind --
  • 4:19 - 4:23
    the social mind, the social
    brain -- depends on.
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    We always think about autism
  • 4:27 - 4:32
    as something that happens
    later on in life.
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    It doesn't; it begins
    with the beginning of life.
  • 4:37 - 4:43
    As babies engage with caregivers,
    they soon realize that, well,
  • 4:43 - 4:47
    there is something between the ears
    that is very important --
  • 4:47 - 4:50
    it's invisible, you can't see it,
    but it's really critical.
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    And that thing is called attention.
  • 4:53 - 4:55
    And they learn soon enough,
  • 4:55 - 4:57
    even before they can utter one word,
  • 4:57 - 5:00
    that they can take that attention
    and move somewhere
  • 5:01 - 5:02
    in order to get things they want.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    They also learn to follow
    other people's gazes,
  • 5:07 - 5:11
    because whatever people are looking at
    is what they are thinking about.
  • 5:13 - 5:17
    And soon enough, they start to learn
    about the meaning of things,
  • 5:17 - 5:19
    because when somebody
    is looking at something
  • 5:19 - 5:21
    or somebody is pointing at something,
  • 5:21 - 5:24
    they're not just getting
    a directional cue.
  • 5:24 - 5:28
    They are getting the other
    person's meaning of that thing,
  • 5:28 - 5:29
    the attitude.
  • 5:29 - 5:34
    And soon enough, they start
    building this body of meanings,
  • 5:34 - 5:38
    but meanings that were acquired
    within the realm of social interaction.
  • 5:39 - 5:40
    Those are meanings that are acquired
  • 5:40 - 5:43
    as part of their shared
    experiences with others.
  • 5:44 - 5:50
    Well, this is a 15-month-old little girl,
  • 5:52 - 5:54
    and she has autism.
  • 5:55 - 6:01
    And I am coming so close to her
    that I am maybe two inches from her face,
  • 6:01 - 6:03
    and she's quite oblivious to me.
  • 6:03 - 6:06
    Imagine if I did that to you,
    came two inches from your face.
  • 6:06 - 6:08
    You'd do probably
    two things, wouldn't you?
  • 6:08 - 6:11
    You would recoil.
    You would call the police.
  • 6:11 - 6:12
    (Laughter)
  • 6:12 - 6:13
    You would do something,
  • 6:13 - 6:18
    because it's literally impossible
    to penetrate somebody's physical space
  • 6:18 - 6:19
    and not get that reaction.
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    We do so, remember,
    intuitively, effortlessly.
  • 6:22 - 6:24
    This is our body wisdom;
  • 6:24 - 6:26
    it's not something mediated
    by our language.
  • 6:26 - 6:28
    Our body just knows that.
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    And we've known that for a long time.
  • 6:31 - 6:34
    And this is not something
    that happens to humans only.
  • 6:34 - 6:37
    It happens to some
    of our phyletic cousins,
  • 6:37 - 6:41
    because if you're a monkey,
    and you look at another monkey,
  • 6:41 - 6:45
    and that monkey has a higher
    hierarchy position than you,
  • 6:45 - 6:49
    and that is considered
    to be a signal or threat,
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    well, you are not going
    to be alive for long.
  • 6:52 - 6:57
    So something that in other species
    are survival mechanisms,
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    without which they
    wouldn't basically live,
  • 7:00 - 7:02
    we bring into the context of human beings,
  • 7:02 - 7:06
    and this is what we need
    to simply act, socially.
  • 7:07 - 7:09
    Now, she is oblivious to me
    and I'm so close to her,
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    and you think, maybe she can see you,
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    maybe she can hear you.
  • 7:13 - 7:15
    Well, a few minutes later,
  • 7:15 - 7:17
    she goes to the corner of the room,
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    and she finds a tiny little piece
    of candy, an M&M.
  • 7:21 - 7:26
    So I could not attract her attention,
  • 7:26 - 7:28
    but something -- a thing -- did.
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    Now, most of us make a big dichotomy
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    between the world of things
    and the world of people.
  • 7:35 - 7:40
    Now, for this girl,
    that division line is not so clear,
  • 7:40 - 7:43
    and the world of people
    is not attracting her
  • 7:43 - 7:44
    as much as we would like.
  • 7:45 - 7:48
    Now, remember that we learn a great deal
    by sharing experiences.
  • 7:49 - 7:55
    What she is doing right now
    is that her path of learning is diverging,
  • 7:55 - 7:57
    moment by moment,
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    as she is isolating herself
    further and further.
  • 8:00 - 8:03
    So we feel sometimes
    that the brain is deterministic,
  • 8:03 - 8:06
    the brain determines
    who we're going to be.
  • 8:06 - 8:09
    But, in fact, the brain
    also becomes who we are,
  • 8:09 - 8:13
    and at the same time
    that her behaviors are taking away
  • 8:13 - 8:15
    from the realm of social interaction,
  • 8:15 - 8:17
    this is what's happening with her mind,
  • 8:17 - 8:19
    and this is what's happening
    with her brain.
  • 8:22 - 8:27
    Well, autism is the most strongly
    genetic condition
  • 8:27 - 8:29
    of all developmental disorders.
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    And it's a brain disorder.
  • 8:33 - 8:36
    It's a disorder that begins
    much prior to the time
  • 8:36 - 8:38
    that the child is born.
  • 8:39 - 8:43
    We now know that there is a very
    broad spectrum of autism.
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    There are those individuals
    who are profoundly intellectually disabled
  • 8:46 - 8:48
    but there are those that are gifted.
  • 8:48 - 8:51
    There are those individuals
    who don't talk at all;
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    there are those individuals
    who talk too much.
  • 8:53 - 8:57
    There are those individuals
    that if you observe them in their school,
  • 8:57 - 9:00
    you see them running the periphery fence
    all the school day if you let them,
  • 9:00 - 9:03
    to those individuals
    who cannot stop coming to you
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    and trying to engage you
    repeatedly, relentlessly,
  • 9:06 - 9:08
    but often in an awkward fashion,
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    without that immediate resonance.
  • 9:12 - 9:16
    Well, this is much more prevalent
    than we thought at the time.
  • 9:16 - 9:17
    When I started in this field,
  • 9:18 - 9:21
    we thought there were four individuals
    with autism per 10,000 --
  • 9:21 - 9:22
    a very rare condition.
  • 9:22 - 9:26
    Well, now we know it's more
    like one in 100.
  • 9:26 - 9:30
    There are millions of individuals
    with autism all around us.
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    The societal cost
    of this condition is huge,
  • 9:35 - 9:38
    in the US alone,
    maybe 35 to 80 billion dollars.
  • 9:38 - 9:39
    And you know what?
  • 9:39 - 9:44
    Most of those funds are associated
    with adolescents and particularly adults
  • 9:44 - 9:46
    who are severely disabled,
  • 9:46 - 9:48
    individuals who need
    wraparound services --
  • 9:48 - 9:50
    services that are very, very intensive.
  • 9:50 - 9:54
    And those services can cost in excess
    of 60,000 to 80,000 dollars a year.
  • 9:55 - 9:58
    Those are individuals who did not
    benefit from early treatment,
  • 9:59 - 10:03
    because now we know
    that autism creates itself
  • 10:03 - 10:07
    as individuals diverge in that pathway
    of learning that I mentioned to you.
  • 10:08 - 10:10
    Were we to be able
    to identify this condition
  • 10:10 - 10:13
    at an earlier point,
    and intervene and treat --
  • 10:14 - 10:18
    I can tell you, this has been probably
    something that has changed my life
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    in the past 10 years,
  • 10:20 - 10:24
    this notion that we can absolutely
    attenuate this condition.
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    Also, we have a window of opportunity,
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    because the brain
    is malleable for just so long,
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    and that window of opportunity
    happens in the first three years of life.
  • 10:34 - 10:37
    It's not that that window
    closes; it doesn't.
  • 10:38 - 10:41
    But it diminishes considerably.
  • 10:41 - 10:44
    And yet, the median age
    of diagnosis in this country
  • 10:45 - 10:46
    is still about five years,
  • 10:46 - 10:49
    and in disadvantaged populations,
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    the populations that don't have
    access to clinical services,
  • 10:52 - 10:54
    rural populations, minorities,
  • 10:55 - 10:58
    the age of diagnosis is later still,
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    which is almost as if I were to tell you
  • 11:00 - 11:03
    that we are condemning those communities
    to have individuals with autism
  • 11:03 - 11:06
    whose condition is going
    to be more severe.
  • 11:07 - 11:09
    So I feel that we have
    a bioethical imperative.
  • 11:10 - 11:11
    The science is there.
  • 11:13 - 11:15
    But no science is of relevance
  • 11:15 - 11:18
    if it doesn't have an impact
    on the community.
  • 11:18 - 11:21
    And we just can't afford
    that missed opportunity,
  • 11:21 - 11:24
    because children with autism
    become adults with autism.
  • 11:25 - 11:29
    And we feel that those things we can do
  • 11:29 - 11:31
    for these children,
    for those families, early on,
  • 11:31 - 11:34
    will have lifetime consequences --
  • 11:34 - 11:38
    for the child, for the family,
    and for the community at large.
  • 11:38 - 11:40
    So this is our view of autism.
  • 11:41 - 11:44
    There are over a hundred genes
    that are associated with autism.
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    In fact, we believe there are going to be
  • 11:46 - 11:50
    something between 300 and 600
    genes associated with autism,
  • 11:50 - 11:53
    and genetic anomalies,
    much more than just genes.
  • 11:54 - 11:57
    And we actually have
    a bit of a question here,
  • 11:58 - 12:01
    because if there are so many
    different causes of autism,
  • 12:01 - 12:06
    how do you go from those liabilities
    to the actual syndrome?
  • 12:06 - 12:07
    Because people like myself,
  • 12:08 - 12:10
    when we walk into a playroom,
  • 12:10 - 12:13
    we recognize a child as having autism.
  • 12:13 - 12:16
    So how do you go from multiple causes
  • 12:16 - 12:18
    to a syndrome that has some homogeneity?
  • 12:19 - 12:22
    And the answer is what lies in between,
  • 12:22 - 12:24
    which is development.
  • 12:24 - 12:29
    And in fact, we are very interested
    in those first two years of life,
  • 12:29 - 12:33
    because those liabilities
    don't necessarily convert into autism.
  • 12:33 - 12:35
    Autism creates itself.
  • 12:35 - 12:39
    Were we to be able to intervene
    during those years of life,
  • 12:40 - 12:45
    we might attenuate for some, and God
    knows, maybe even prevent for others.
  • 12:46 - 12:48
    So how do we do that?
  • 12:48 - 12:51
    How do we enter that feeling of resonance,
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    how do we enter another person's being?
  • 12:56 - 12:59
    I remember when I interacted
    with that 15-month-old,
  • 12:59 - 13:01
    the thing that came to my mind was,
  • 13:01 - 13:03
    "How do you come into her world?
  • 13:04 - 13:07
    Is she thinking about me?
    Is she thinking about others?"
  • 13:08 - 13:11
    Well, it's hard to do that,
  • 13:11 - 13:13
    so we had to create the technologies.
  • 13:13 - 13:16
    We had to basically step inside a body.
  • 13:16 - 13:19
    We had to see the world through her eyes.
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    And so in the past many years,
  • 13:22 - 13:24
    we've been building these new technologies
  • 13:24 - 13:26
    that are based on eye tracking.
  • 13:26 - 13:31
    We can see, moment by moment,
    what children are engaging with.
  • 13:33 - 13:34
    This is my colleague, Warren Jones,
  • 13:34 - 13:38
    with whom we've been building
    these methods, these studies,
  • 13:38 - 13:39
    for the past 12 years.
  • 13:39 - 13:43
    And you see there a happy five-month-old,
  • 13:43 - 13:48
    a five-month little boy
    who is going to watch things
  • 13:48 - 13:50
    that are brought from his world:
  • 13:51 - 13:53
    his mom, the caregiver,
  • 13:53 - 13:58
    but also experiences that he would have
    were he to be in his daycare.
  • 13:58 - 14:02
    What we want is to embrace that world
    and bring it into our laboratory,
  • 14:02 - 14:04
    but in order for us to do that,
  • 14:04 - 14:09
    we had to create
    these very sophisticated measures,
  • 14:09 - 14:12
    measures of how people, how little babies,
  • 14:12 - 14:17
    how newborns, engage
    with the world, moment by moment.
  • 14:17 - 14:19
    What is important and what is not.
  • 14:20 - 14:22
    Well, we created those measures,
  • 14:22 - 14:26
    and here, what you see
    is what we call a funnel of attention.
  • 14:26 - 14:28
    You're watching a video --
  • 14:28 - 14:31
    those frames are separated
    by about a second --
  • 14:31 - 14:35
    through the eyes of 35 typically
    developing two-year-olds.
  • 14:35 - 14:38
    And we freeze one frame,
  • 14:38 - 14:41
    and this is what the typical
    children are doing.
  • 14:42 - 14:46
    In this scan pass, in green here,
    are two-year-olds with autism.
  • 14:46 - 14:51
    So on that frame, the children
    who are typical are watching this,
  • 14:53 - 14:55
    the emotion of expression
    of that little boy
  • 14:55 - 14:58
    as he's fighting a little bit
    with the little girl.
  • 14:58 - 15:00
    What are the children with autism doing?
  • 15:00 - 15:03
    They are focusing on the revolving door,
  • 15:04 - 15:05
    opening and shutting.
  • 15:06 - 15:09
    Well, I can tell you that this divergence
    that you're seeing here
  • 15:09 - 15:12
    doesn't happen only
    in our five-minute experiment.
  • 15:12 - 15:15
    It happens moment by moment
    in their real lives,
  • 15:16 - 15:21
    and their minds are being formed
    and their brains are being specialized
  • 15:21 - 15:25
    in something other than what is happening
    with their typical peers.
  • 15:25 - 15:32
    Well, we took a construct
    from our pediatrician friends,
  • 15:32 - 15:33
    the concept of growth charts --
  • 15:33 - 15:36
    you know, when you take
    a child to the pediatrician,
  • 15:36 - 15:39
    and you have physical height and weight.
  • 15:40 - 15:42
    Well, we decided we were going
    to create growth charts
  • 15:43 - 15:44
    of social engagement.
  • 15:45 - 15:48
    We sought children
    from the time they're born.
  • 15:48 - 15:51
    What you see here on the x-axis
  • 15:51 - 15:56
    is two, three, four, five,
    six months and nine,
  • 15:56 - 15:57
    until about the age of 24 months.
  • 15:57 - 16:00
    This is the percent of their viewing time
  • 16:00 - 16:02
    that they're focusing on people's eyes,
  • 16:02 - 16:04
    and this is their growth chart.
  • 16:05 - 16:07
    They start over here --
    they love people's eyes --
  • 16:07 - 16:09
    and it remains quite stable.
  • 16:10 - 16:13
    It sort of goes up a little bit
    in those initial months.
  • 16:14 - 16:18
    Now, let's see what's happening
    with babies who became autistic.
  • 16:18 - 16:20
    It's something very different.
  • 16:21 - 16:23
    It starts way up here,
    but then it's a free fall.
  • 16:24 - 16:28
    It's very much like they brought
    into this world the reflex
  • 16:28 - 16:31
    that orients them to people,
    but it has no traction.
  • 16:32 - 16:34
    It's almost as if that stimulus -- you --
  • 16:35 - 16:38
    you're not exerting
    influence on what happens
  • 16:38 - 16:40
    as they navigate their daily lives.
  • 16:42 - 16:48
    Now, we thought those data
    were so powerful, in a way,
  • 16:49 - 16:52
    that we wanted to see what happened
    in the first six months of life,
  • 16:52 - 16:56
    because if you interact
    with a two- and a three-month-old,
  • 16:56 - 16:59
    you'd be surprised
    by how social those babies are.
  • 17:00 - 17:03
    And what we see
    in the first six months of life
  • 17:03 - 17:08
    is that those two groups
    can be segregated very easily.
  • 17:09 - 17:12
    And using these kinds
    of measures and many others,
  • 17:12 - 17:15
    what we found out
    is that our science could, in fact,
  • 17:15 - 17:18
    identify this condition early on.
  • 17:18 - 17:21
    We didn't have to wait
    for the behaviors of autism
  • 17:21 - 17:24
    to emerge in the second year of life.
  • 17:25 - 17:29
    If we measured things that are,
    evolutionarily, highly conserved,
  • 17:29 - 17:32
    and developmentally very early-emerging --
  • 17:32 - 17:34
    things that are online
    from the first weeks of life --
  • 17:34 - 17:36
    we could push the detection of autism
  • 17:36 - 17:39
    all the way to those first months,
  • 17:39 - 17:41
    and that's what we are doing now.
  • 17:43 - 17:46
    Now, we can create
    the very best technologies
  • 17:46 - 17:49
    and the very best methods
    to identify the children,
  • 17:50 - 17:53
    but this would be for naught
    if we didn't have an impact
  • 17:53 - 17:56
    on what happens in their reality
    in the community.
  • 17:56 - 17:58
    Now we want those devices, of course,
  • 17:58 - 18:01
    to be deployed by those
    who are in the trenches --
  • 18:01 - 18:06
    our colleagues, the primary care
    physicians, who see every child --
  • 18:07 - 18:09
    and we need to transform
    those technologies
  • 18:09 - 18:12
    into something that is going
    to add value to their practice,
  • 18:12 - 18:14
    because they have to see so many children.
  • 18:14 - 18:18
    And we want to do that universally
    so that we don't miss any child.
  • 18:18 - 18:20
    But this would be immoral
  • 18:21 - 18:26
    if we also did not have an infrastructure
    for intervention, for treatment.
  • 18:27 - 18:31
    We need to be able to work
    with the families, support the families,
  • 18:31 - 18:34
    to manage those first years with them.
  • 18:35 - 18:37
    We need to be able to really go
  • 18:37 - 18:41
    from universal screening
    to universal access to treatment,
  • 18:41 - 18:44
    because those treatments
    are going to change
  • 18:44 - 18:47
    these children's
    and those families' lives.
  • 18:49 - 18:55
    Now, when we think about what we [can]
    do in those first years,
  • 18:55 - 19:00
    I can tell you, having been
    in this field for so long,
  • 19:01 - 19:03
    one feels really rejuvenated.
  • 19:03 - 19:08
    There is a sense that the science
    that one worked on
  • 19:08 - 19:11
    can actually have an impact on realities,
  • 19:11 - 19:14
    preventing, in fact, those experiences
  • 19:14 - 19:17
    that I really started
    in my journey in this field.
  • 19:18 - 19:21
    I thought at the time
    that this was an intractable condition.
  • 19:21 - 19:25
    No longer. We can do
    a great deal of things.
  • 19:25 - 19:28
    And the idea is not to cure autism.
  • 19:28 - 19:29
    That's not the idea.
  • 19:30 - 19:32
    What we want is to make sure
  • 19:32 - 19:35
    that those individuals
    with autism can be free
  • 19:35 - 19:39
    from the devastating consequences
    that come with it at times,
  • 19:39 - 19:42
    the profound intellectual disabilities,
    the lack of language,
  • 19:42 - 19:45
    the profound, profound isolation.
  • 19:46 - 19:48
    We feel that individuals
    with autism, in fact,
  • 19:48 - 19:51
    have a very special
    perspective on the world,
  • 19:51 - 19:52
    and we need diversity.
  • 19:53 - 19:57
    And they can work extremely well
    in some areas of strength:
  • 19:57 - 20:00
    predictable situations,
    situations that can be defined.
  • 20:00 - 20:03
    Because after all,
    they learn about the world
  • 20:03 - 20:05
    almost, like, about it,
  • 20:05 - 20:08
    rather than learning
    how to function in it.
  • 20:08 - 20:12
    But this is a strength if you're working,
    for example, in technology.
  • 20:13 - 20:16
    And there are those individuals
    who have incredible artistic abilities.
  • 20:16 - 20:19
    We want them to be free to do that.
  • 20:19 - 20:22
    We want that the next generations
    of individuals with autism
  • 20:22 - 20:25
    will be able not only
    to express their strengths,
  • 20:25 - 20:27
    but to fulfill their promise.
  • 20:27 - 20:29
    Well, thank you for listening to me.
  • 20:29 - 20:33
    (Applause)
  • 20:35 - 20:37
    Host: Dr Klin, thank you.
  • 20:37 - 20:41
    You know, this talk is generating
    quite a lot of talk
  • 20:41 - 20:43
    all over the web right now,
  • 20:43 - 20:45
    a lot of tweeting going on,
  • 20:45 - 20:48
    and obviously a lot of interest
    around the issue of autism.
  • 20:48 - 20:52
    I'm curious to know whether
    there are some new caregiving norms
  • 20:52 - 20:55
    that you see - you mentioned
    that early intervention is the key,
  • 20:55 - 20:57
    but if there isn't early diagnoses,
  • 20:57 - 21:00
    is there a look that caregivers
    should perhaps be -
  • 21:00 - 21:04
    evolving into new ways of caring
    for children that might be helpful ?
  • 21:04 - 21:08
    Ami Klin: Well, there are ways
    of identifying children early.
  • 21:08 - 21:11
    The fact that there is a slow
    uptake of that
  • 21:11 - 21:14
    has something to do with the fact
    that people need to know how bad.
  • 21:14 - 21:17
    That we can do a great deal
    for these children and families.
  • 21:17 - 21:19
    But the interesting thing,
  • 21:19 - 21:22
    is that the forms of treatment
    that are available now
  • 21:22 - 21:26
    are not forms of treatment
    that would be taking place in a center.
  • 21:26 - 21:28
    They take place in the homes.
  • 21:28 - 21:31
    Basically, what those treatments
    are doing,
  • 21:31 - 21:34
    is potentiating what every caregiver does.
  • 21:34 - 21:35
    We need to make sure
  • 21:35 - 21:38
    that those signals,
    the communication signals,
  • 21:38 - 21:39
    the social engagement,
  • 21:39 - 21:42
    that that happens not
    in a treatment session
  • 21:42 - 21:45
    that would take place
    for like 30 minutes a day,
  • 21:46 - 21:49
    we basically need every moment
    in these childrens' lives,
  • 21:49 - 21:50
    to be a treatment moment.
  • 21:51 - 21:54
    And for that, we need to both
    support and train parents.
  • 21:54 - 21:57
    And that's the way
    those early forms of treatment take place.
  • 21:57 - 21:59
    So there is plenty to do out there.
  • 21:59 - 22:02
    Host: Well, it looks
    like people are interested in that.
  • 22:02 - 22:04
    Thank you very much, Dr Klin.
    Pleasure to have you.
  • 22:04 - 22:05
    (Applause)
Title:
A new way to diagnose autism | Ami Klin | TEDxPeachtree
Description:

Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder can improve the lives of everyone affected, but the complex network of causes make it incredibly difficult to predict. Ami Klin describes a new early detection method that uses eye-tracking technologies to gauge babies' social engagement skills and reliably measure their risk of developing autism.

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at http://ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
22:08

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions