Return to Video

A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory.

  • 0:00 - 0:02
    Steve Ramirez: My first
    year of grad school,
  • 0:02 - 0:04
    I found myself in my bedroom
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    eating lots of Ben & Jerry's
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    watching some trashy TV
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    and maybe, maybe listening
    to Taylor Swift.
  • 0:11 - 0:13
    I had just gone through a breakup.
  • 0:13 - 0:14
    (Laughter)
  • 0:14 - 0:17
    So for the longest time, all I would do
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    is recall the memory of this
    person over and over again,
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    wishing that I could get
    rid of that gut-wrenching,
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    visceral "blah" feeling.
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    Now, as it turns out,
    I'm a neuroscientist,
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    so I knew that the memory of that person
  • 0:30 - 0:33
    and the awful, emotional undertones
    that color in that memory,
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    are largely mediated
    by separate brain systems.
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    And so I thought, what if we could
    go into the brain
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    and edit out that nauseating feeling
  • 0:40 - 0:43
    but while keeping the memory
    of that person intact?
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    Then I realized, maybe
    that's a little bit lofty for now.
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    So what if we could start
    off by going into the brain
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    and just finding a single
    memory to begin with?
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    Could we jump-start
    that memory back to life,
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    maybe even play with the contents
    of that memory?
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    All that said, there is one person
    in the entire world right now
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    that I really hope is not
    watching this talk.
  • 1:02 - 1:06
    (Laughter)
  • 1:06 - 1:09
    So there is a catch. There is a catch.
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    These ideas probably remind
    you of "Total Recall,"
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    "Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind,"
  • 1:14 - 1:15
    or of "Inception."
  • 1:15 - 1:17
    But the movie stars that we work with
  • 1:17 - 1:19
    are the celebrities of the lab.
  • 1:19 - 1:20
    Xu Liu: Test mice.
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    (Laughter)
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    As neuroscientists, we work
    in the lab with mice
  • 1:25 - 1:28
    trying to understand how memory works.
  • 1:28 - 1:31
    And today, we hope
    to convince you that now
  • 1:31 - 1:34
    we are actually able to activate
    a memory in the brain
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    at the speed of light.
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    To do this, there's only two simple
    steps to follow.
  • 1:39 - 1:43
    First, you find and label
    a memory in the brain,
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    and then you activate it with a switch.
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    As simple as that.
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    (Laughter)
  • 1:50 - 1:51
    SR: Are you convinced?
  • 1:51 - 1:55
    So, turns out finding a memory
    in the brain isn't all that easy.
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    XL: Indeed. This is way more
    difficult than, let's say,
  • 1:58 - 2:00
    finding a needle in a haystack,
  • 2:00 - 2:03
    because at least, you know,
    the needle is still something
  • 2:03 - 2:05
    you can physically put your fingers on.
  • 2:05 - 2:07
    But memory is not.
  • 2:07 - 2:10
    And also, there's way
    more cells in your brain
  • 2:11 - 2:16
    than the number of straws
    in a typical haystack.
  • 2:16 - 2:18
    So yeah, this task does
    seem to be daunting.
  • 2:18 - 2:22
    But luckily, we got help
    from the brain itself.
  • 2:22 - 2:25
    It turned out that all we need
    to do is basically
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    to let the brain form a memory,
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    and then the brain will tell
    us which cells are involved
  • 2:30 - 2:32
    in that particular memory.
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    SR: So what was going on in my brain
  • 2:35 - 2:37
    while I was recalling the memory of an ex?
  • 2:37 - 2:39
    If you were to just completely
    ignore human ethics for a second
  • 2:39 - 2:41
    and slice up my brain right now,
  • 2:41 - 2:43
    you would see that there
    was an amazing number
  • 2:43 - 2:46
    of brain regions that were active
    while recalling that memory.
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    Now one brain region
    that would be robustly active
  • 2:49 - 2:51
    in particular is called the hippocampus,
  • 2:51 - 2:53
    which for decades has
    been implicated in processing
  • 2:53 - 2:56
    the kinds of memories
    that we hold near and dear,
  • 2:56 - 2:58
    which also makes it
    an ideal target to go into
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    and to try and find and maybe
    reactivate a memory.
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    XL: When you zoom in into the hippocampus,
  • 3:03 - 3:06
    of course you will see lots of cells,
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    but we are able to find
    which cells are involved
  • 3:09 - 3:10
    in a particular memory,
  • 3:10 - 3:13
    because whenever a cell is active,
  • 3:13 - 3:14
    like when it's forming a memory,
  • 3:14 - 3:18
    it will also leave a footprint
    that will later allow us to know
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    these cells are recently active.
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    SR: So the same way
    that building lights at night
  • 3:23 - 3:27
    let you know that somebody's probably
    working there at any given moment,
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    in a very real sense, there
    are biological sensors
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    within a cell that are turned on
  • 3:31 - 3:33
    only when that cell was just working.
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    They're sort of biological
    windows that light up
  • 3:36 - 3:38
    to let us know that that cell
    was just active.
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    XL: So we clipped part of this sensor,
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    and attached that to a switch
    to control the cells,
  • 3:43 - 3:47
    and we packed this switch
    into an engineered virus
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    and injected that into the brain
    of the mice.
  • 3:50 - 3:52
    So whenever a memory is being formed,
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    any active cells for that memory
  • 3:55 - 3:57
    will also have this switch installed.
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    SR: So here is what the hippocampus
    looks like
  • 4:00 - 4:02
    after forming a fear memory, for example.
  • 4:02 - 4:04
    The sea of blue that you see here
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    are densely packed brain cells,
  • 4:06 - 4:07
    but the green brain cells,
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    the green brain cells
    are the ones that are holding on
  • 4:10 - 4:11
    to a specific fear memory.
  • 4:11 - 4:13
    So you are looking at the crystallization
  • 4:13 - 4:16
    of the fleeting formation of fear.
  • 4:16 - 4:19
    You're actually looking
    at the cross-section of a memory right now.
  • 4:19 - 4:22
    XL: Now, for the switch
    we have been talking about,
  • 4:22 - 4:25
    ideally, the switch has
    to act really fast.
  • 4:25 - 4:27
    It shouldn't take minutes
    or hours to work.
  • 4:27 - 4:31
    It should act at the speed
    of the brain, in milliseconds.
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    SR: So what do you think, Xu?
  • 4:33 - 4:35
    Could we use, let's say,
    pharmacological drugs
  • 4:36 - 4:37
    to activate or inactivate brain cells?
  • 4:37 - 4:41
    XL: Nah. Drugs are pretty messy.
    They spread everywhere.
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    And also it takes them
    forever to act on cells.
  • 4:44 - 4:48
    So it will not allow us
    to control a memory in real time.
  • 4:48 - 4:52
    So Steve, how about let's zap
    the brain with electricity?
  • 4:52 - 4:55
    SR: So electricity is pretty fast,
  • 4:55 - 4:56
    but we probably wouldn't
    be able to target it
  • 4:56 - 4:59
    to just the specific cells
    that hold onto a memory,
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    and we'd probably fry the brain.
  • 5:01 - 5:04
    XL: Oh. That's true.
    So it looks like, hmm,
  • 5:04 - 5:06
    indeed we need to find a better way
  • 5:06 - 5:10
    to impact the brain at the speed of light.
  • 5:10 - 5:15
    SR: So it just so happens that light
    travels at the speed of light.
  • 5:15 - 5:18
    So maybe we could activate
    or inactive memories
  • 5:18 - 5:20
    by just using light --
  • 5:20 - 5:21
    XL: That's pretty fast.
  • 5:21 - 5:23
    SR: -- and because normally brain cells
  • 5:23 - 5:25
    don't respond to pulses of light,
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    so those that would respond
    to pulses of light
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    are those that contain
    a light-sensitive switch.
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    Now to do that, first we need
    to trick brain cells
  • 5:31 - 5:32
    to respond to laser beams.
  • 5:32 - 5:34
    XL: Yep. You heard it right.
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    We are trying to shoot
    lasers into the brain.
  • 5:36 - 5:37
    (Laughter)
  • 5:37 - 5:41
    SR: And the technique that lets
    us do that is optogenetics.
  • 5:41 - 5:44
    Optogenetics gave us this
    light switch that we can use
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    to turn brain cells on or off,
  • 5:46 - 5:48
    and the name of that switch
    is channelrhodopsin,
  • 5:48 - 5:51
    seen here as these green dots
    attached to this brain cell.
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    You can think of channelrhodopsin
    as a sort of light-sensitive switch
  • 5:54 - 5:56
    that can be artificially
    installed in brain cells
  • 5:57 - 5:58
    so that now we can use that switch
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    to activate or inactivate the brain
    cell simply by clicking it,
  • 6:01 - 6:04
    and in this case we click
    it on with pulses of light.
  • 6:04 - 6:08
    XL: So we attach this light-sensitive
    switch of channelrhodopsin
  • 6:08 - 6:10
    to the sensor we've been talking about
  • 6:10 - 6:12
    and inject this into the brain.
  • 6:12 - 6:16
    So whenever a memory is being formed,
  • 6:16 - 6:18
    any active cell for that particular memory
  • 6:18 - 6:21
    will also have this light-sensitive
    switch installed in it
  • 6:21 - 6:24
    so that we can control these cells
  • 6:24 - 6:28
    by the flipping of a laser
    just like this one you see.
  • 6:28 - 6:31
    SR: So let's put all of this
    to the test now.
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    What we can do is we can take our mice
  • 6:33 - 6:36
    and then we can put them in a box
    that looks exactly like this box here,
  • 6:36 - 6:38
    and then we can give them
    a very mild foot shock
  • 6:38 - 6:40
    so that they form a fear
    memory of this box.
  • 6:40 - 6:42
    They learn that something
    bad happened here.
  • 6:42 - 6:45
    Now with our system,
    the cells that are active
  • 6:45 - 6:47
    in the hippocampus
    in the making of this memory,
  • 6:47 - 6:50
    only those cells will now
    contain channelrhodopsin.
  • 6:50 - 6:53
    XL: When you are as small as a mouse,
  • 6:53 - 6:57
    it feels as if the whole
    world is trying to get you.
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    So your best response of defense
  • 6:59 - 7:01
    is trying to be undetected.
  • 7:01 - 7:03
    Whenever a mouse is in fear,
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    it will show this very typical behavior
  • 7:05 - 7:07
    by staying at one corner of the box,
  • 7:07 - 7:10
    trying to not move any part of its body,
  • 7:10 - 7:13
    and this posture is called freezing.
  • 7:13 - 7:17
    So if a mouse remembers that something
    bad happened in this box,
  • 7:17 - 7:20
    and when we put them
    back into the same box,
  • 7:20 - 7:22
    it will basically show freezing
  • 7:22 - 7:24
    because it doesn't want to be detected
  • 7:24 - 7:27
    by any potential threats in this box.
  • 7:27 - 7:28
    SR: So you can think of freezing as,
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    you're walking down the street
    minding your own business,
  • 7:30 - 7:32
    and then out of nowhere
    you almost run into
  • 7:32 - 7:34
    an ex-girlfriend or ex-boyfriend,
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    and now those terrifying two seconds
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    where you start thinking, "What do I do?
    Do I say hi?
  • 7:38 - 7:40
    Do I shake their hand? Do
    I turn around and run away?
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    Do I sit here and pretend
    like I don't exist?"
  • 7:42 - 7:45
    Those kinds of fleeting thoughts
    that physically incapacitate you,
  • 7:45 - 7:47
    that temporarily give you
    that deer-in-headlights look.
  • 7:47 - 7:51
    XL: However, if you put the mouse
    in a completely different
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    new box, like the next one,
  • 7:54 - 7:56
    it will not be afraid of this box
  • 7:56 - 8:01
    because there's no reason that it
    will be afraid of this new environment.
  • 8:01 - 8:04
    But what if we put
    the mouse in this new box
  • 8:04 - 8:08
    but at the same time,
    we activate the fear memory
  • 8:08 - 8:10
    using lasers just like we did before?
  • 8:10 - 8:13
    Are we going to bring back the fear memory
  • 8:13 - 8:17
    for the first box into this
    completely new environment?
  • 8:17 - 8:20
    SR: All right,
    and here's the million-dollar experiment.
  • 8:20 - 8:23
    Now to bring back to life
    the memory of that day,
  • 8:23 - 8:25
    I remember that the Red Sox had just won,
  • 8:25 - 8:27
    it was a green spring day,
  • 8:27 - 8:29
    perfect for going up and down the river
  • 8:29 - 8:31
    and then maybe going to the North End
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    to get some cannolis, #justsaying.
  • 8:33 - 8:36
    Now Xu and I, on the other hand,
  • 8:36 - 8:39
    were in a completely windowless black room
  • 8:39 - 8:43
    not making any ocular movement
    that even remotely resembles an eye blink
  • 8:43 - 8:45
    because our eyes were fixed
    onto a computer screen.
  • 8:45 - 8:48
    We were looking at this mouse
    here trying to activate a memory
  • 8:48 - 8:50
    for the first time using our technique.
  • 8:50 - 8:52
    XL: And this is what we saw.
  • 8:52 - 8:55
    When we first put the mouse into this box,
  • 8:55 - 8:58
    it's exploring, sniffing
    around, walking around,
  • 8:58 - 8:59
    minding its own business,
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    because actually by nature,
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    mice are pretty curious animals.
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    They want to know, what's going
    on in this new box?
  • 9:06 - 9:07
    It's interesting.
  • 9:07 - 9:11
    But the moment we turned
    on the laser, like you see now,
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    all of a sudden the mouse
    entered this freezing mode.
  • 9:14 - 9:18
    It stayed here and tried not
    to move any part of its body.
  • 9:18 - 9:20
    Clearly it's freezing.
  • 9:20 - 9:22
    So indeed, it looks
    like we are able to bring back
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    the fear memory for the first box
  • 9:24 - 9:28
    in this completely new environment.
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    While watching this, Steve and I
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    are as shocked as the mouse itself.
  • 9:32 - 9:33
    (Laughter)
  • 9:33 - 9:37
    So after the experiment,
    the two of us just left the room
  • 9:37 - 9:38
    without saying anything.
  • 9:38 - 9:42
    After a kind of long,
    awkward period of time,
  • 9:42 - 9:44
    Steve broke the silence.
  • 9:44 - 9:46
    SR: "Did that just work?"
  • 9:46 - 9:49
    XL: "Yes," I said. "Indeed it worked!"
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    We're really excited about this.
  • 9:51 - 9:54
    And then we published our findings
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    in the journal Nature.
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    Ever since the publication of our work,
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    we've been receiving numerous comments
  • 10:01 - 10:03
    from all over the Internet.
  • 10:03 - 10:06
    Maybe we can take a look at some of those.
  • 10:06 - 10:09
    ["OMGGGGG FINALLY... so much more to come, virtual reality, neural manipulation, visual dream emulation...
    neural coding, 'writing and re-writing of memories', mental illnesses. Ahhh the future is awesome"]
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    SR: So the first thing
    that you'll notice is that people
  • 10:11 - 10:14
    have really strong opinions
    about this kind of work.
  • 10:14 - 10:16
    Now I happen to completely
    agree with the optimism
  • 10:16 - 10:17
    of this first quote,
  • 10:17 - 10:20
    because on a scale
    of zero to Morgan Freeman's voice,
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    it happens to be
    one of the most evocative accolades
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    that I've heard come our way.
  • 10:24 - 10:26
    (Laughter)
  • 10:26 - 10:28
    But as you'll see, it's not
    the only opinion that's out there.
  • 10:28 - 10:29
    ["This scares the hell out of me... What if they could do that easily
    in humans in a couple of years?! OH MY GOD WE'RE DOOMED"]
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    XL: Indeed, if we take
    a look at the second one,
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    I think we can all agree that it's, meh,
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    probably not as positive.
  • 10:36 - 10:38
    But this also reminds us that,
  • 10:38 - 10:40
    although we are still working with mice,
  • 10:40 - 10:44
    it's probably a good idea
    to start thinking and discussing
  • 10:44 - 10:47
    about the possible ethical ramifications
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    of memory control.
  • 10:49 - 10:51
    SR: Now, in the spirit of the third quote,
  • 10:51 - 10:53
    we want to tell you about a recent
    project that we've been
  • 10:53 - 10:55
    working on in lab that we've called
    Project Inception.
  • 10:55 - 10:59
    ["They should make a movie about this. Where they plant ideas into peoples minds,
    so they can control them for their own personal gain. We'll call it: Inception."]
  • 10:59 - 11:02
    So we reasoned that now
    that we can reactivate a memory,
  • 11:02 - 11:05
    what if we do so but then begin
    to tinker with that memory?
  • 11:05 - 11:08
    Could we possibly even turn
    it into a false memory?
  • 11:08 - 11:12
    XL: So all memory
    is sophisticated and dynamic,
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    but if just for simplicity,
    let's imagine memory
  • 11:15 - 11:17
    as a movie clip.
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    So far what we've told you
    is basically we can control
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    this "play" button of the clip
  • 11:21 - 11:26
    so that we can play this video
    clip any time, anywhere.
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    But is there a possibility
    that we can actually get
  • 11:28 - 11:31
    inside the brain and edit this movie clip
  • 11:31 - 11:34
    so that we can make it
    different from the original?
  • 11:34 - 11:36
    Yes we can.
  • 11:36 - 11:38
    Turned out that all we need
    to do is basically
  • 11:39 - 11:43
    reactivate a memory using
    lasers just like we did before,
  • 11:43 - 11:46
    but at the same time,
    if we present new information
  • 11:46 - 11:50
    and allow this new information
    to incorporate into this old memory,
  • 11:50 - 11:53
    this will change the memory.
  • 11:53 - 11:56
    It's sort of like making a remix tape.
  • 11:56 - 11:59
    SR: So how do we do this?
  • 11:59 - 12:01
    Rather than finding a fear
    memory in the brain,
  • 12:01 - 12:03
    we can start by taking our animals,
  • 12:03 - 12:06
    and let's say we put them in a blue
    box like this blue box here
  • 12:06 - 12:08
    and we find the brain cells
    that represent that blue box
  • 12:08 - 12:11
    and we trick them to respond
    to pulses of light
  • 12:11 - 12:12
    exactly like we had said before.
  • 12:12 - 12:14
    Now the next day, we can take
    our animals and place them
  • 12:14 - 12:17
    in a red box that they've never
    experienced before.
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    We can shoot light
    into the brain to reactivate
  • 12:19 - 12:21
    the memory of the blue box.
  • 12:21 - 12:23
    So what would happen here
    if, while the animal
  • 12:23 - 12:25
    is recalling the memory of the blue box,
  • 12:25 - 12:28
    we gave it a couple of mild foot shocks?
  • 12:28 - 12:30
    So here we're trying to artificially
    make an association
  • 12:30 - 12:32
    between the memory of the blue box
  • 12:32 - 12:34
    and the foot shocks themselves.
  • 12:34 - 12:35
    We're just trying to connect the two.
  • 12:36 - 12:37
    So to test if we had done so,
  • 12:37 - 12:38
    we can take our animals once again
  • 12:38 - 12:40
    and place them back in the blue box.
  • 12:40 - 12:43
    Again, we had just reactivated
    the memory of the blue box
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    while the animal got a couple
    of mild foot shocks,
  • 12:45 - 12:48
    and now the animal suddenly freezes.
  • 12:48 - 12:51
    It's as though it's recalling being
    mildly shocked in this environment
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    even though that never actually happened.
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    So it formed a false memory,
  • 12:56 - 12:58
    because it's falsely
    fearing an environment
  • 12:58 - 12:59
    where, technically speaking,
  • 12:59 - 13:01
    nothing bad actually happened to it.
  • 13:01 - 13:04
    XL: So, so far we are only talking about
  • 13:04 - 13:06
    this light-controlled "on" switch.
  • 13:06 - 13:09
    In fact, we also have
    a light-controlled "off" switch,
  • 13:09 - 13:11
    and it's very easy to imagine that
  • 13:11 - 13:14
    by installing this
    light-controlled "off" switch,
  • 13:14 - 13:19
    we can also turn off a memory,
    any time, anywhere.
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    So everything
    we've been talking about today
  • 13:22 - 13:26
    is based on this philosophically
    charged principle of neuroscience
  • 13:26 - 13:31
    that the mind, with its seemingly
    mysterious properties,
  • 13:31 - 13:34
    is actually made of physical
    stuff that we can tinker with.
  • 13:34 - 13:36
    SR: And for me personally,
  • 13:36 - 13:37
    I see a world where we can reactivate
  • 13:37 - 13:39
    any kind of memory that we'd like.
  • 13:39 - 13:43
    I also see a world where we can
    erase unwanted memories.
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    Now, I even see a world
    where editing memories
  • 13:45 - 13:46
    is something of a reality,
  • 13:46 - 13:48
    because we're living in a time
    where it's possible
  • 13:48 - 13:50
    to pluck questions from the tree
    of science fiction
  • 13:50 - 13:52
    and to ground them
    in experimental reality.
  • 13:52 - 13:54
    XL: Nowadays, people in the lab
  • 13:54 - 13:57
    and people in other
    groups all over the world
  • 13:57 - 14:01
    are using similar methods
    to activate or edit memories,
  • 14:01 - 14:04
    whether that's old or new,
    positive or negative,
  • 14:04 - 14:07
    all sorts of memories so
    that we can understand
  • 14:07 - 14:09
    how memory works.
  • 14:09 - 14:11
    SR: For example, one group in our lab
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    was able to find the brain cells
    that make up a fear memory
  • 14:13 - 14:16
    and converted them into a pleasurable
    memory, just like that.
  • 14:16 - 14:19
    That's exactly what I mean about editing
    these kinds of processes.
  • 14:19 - 14:22
    Now one dude in lab
    was even able to reactivate
  • 14:22 - 14:23
    memories of female mice in male mice,
  • 14:24 - 14:26
    which rumor has it
    is a pleasurable experience.
  • 14:26 - 14:31
    XL: Indeed, we are living
    in a very exciting moment
  • 14:31 - 14:34
    where science doesn't have
    any arbitrary speed limits
  • 14:34 - 14:38
    but is only bound by our own imagination.
  • 14:38 - 14:40
    SR: And finally, what do
    we make of all this?
  • 14:40 - 14:42
    How do we push this technology forward?
  • 14:42 - 14:44
    These are the questions
    that should not remain
  • 14:44 - 14:45
    just inside the lab,
  • 14:45 - 14:48
    and so one goal of today's talk
    was to bring everybody
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    up to speed with the kind
    of stuff that's possible
  • 14:50 - 14:51
    in modern neuroscience,
  • 14:52 - 14:53
    but now, just as importantly,
  • 14:53 - 14:56
    to actively engage everybody
    in this conversation.
  • 14:56 - 14:59
    So let's think together as a team
    about what this all means
  • 14:59 - 15:01
    and where we can and should go from here,
  • 15:01 - 15:03
    because Xu and I think we all have
  • 15:03 - 15:06
    some really big decisions ahead of us.
  • 15:06 - 15:07
    Thank you. XL: Thank you.
  • 15:07 - 15:09
    (Applause)
Title:
A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory.
Speaker:
Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu
Description:

Can we edit the content of our memories? It’s a sci-fi-tinged question that Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu are asking in their lab at MIT. Essentially, the pair shoot a laser beam into the brain of a living mouse to activate and manipulate its memory. In this unexpectedly amusing talk they share not only how, but -- more importantly -- why they do this. (Filmed at TEDxBoston.)

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
15:25

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions