Return to Video

The secret social lives of bats: Nicolas Perony at TEDxZurich

  • 0:19 - 0:21
    Science.
  • 0:21 - 0:24
    Science has allowed us to know so much
  • 0:24 - 0:27
    about the far reaches of the universe,
  • 0:27 - 0:29
    which is at the same time
  • 0:29 - 0:32
    tremendously important
    and extremely remote.
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    And yet, much closer,
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    much more directly related to us,
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    there are many things
    we don't fully understand.
  • 0:39 - 0:43
    One of them is the extraordinary
    social complexity
  • 0:43 - 0:45
    of the animals around us.
  • 0:45 - 0:49
    And today I want to tell you
    a few stories of animal complexity.
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    But first, what do we call complexity?
  • 0:52 - 0:54
    What is complex?
  • 0:54 - 0:57
    Well, complex is not complicated.
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    Something complicated
    comprises many small parts,
  • 1:00 - 1:03
    all different, and each of them
  • 1:03 - 1:06
    has its own precise role
    in the machinery.
  • 1:06 - 1:09
    On the opposite, a complex system
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    is made of many, many similar parts
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    and it is their interaction
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    that produces the globally
    coherent behavior.
  • 1:17 - 1:21
    Complex systems have
    many interacting parts,
  • 1:21 - 1:24
    which behave according
    to simple individual rules
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    and this results in emergent properties.
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    The behavior of the system as a whole
  • 1:29 - 1:33
    cannot be predicted
    from the individual rules only.
  • 1:33 - 1:35
    As Aristotle wrote:
  • 1:35 - 1:38
    The whole is greater
    than the sum of its parts.
  • 1:38 - 1:40
    But, from Aristotle, let's move onto
  • 1:40 - 1:44
    a more concrete example of complex system.
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    These are Scottish Terriers.
  • 1:46 - 1:50
    In the beginning,
    the system is disorganised.
  • 1:50 - 1:54
    Then comes a perturbation: milk.
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    Every individual starts pushing
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    in one direction -- (Laughter)
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    -- and this is what happens.
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    The pinwheel is an emergent property
  • 2:04 - 2:06
    of the interactions between puppies,
  • 2:06 - 2:10
    whose only rule is to try
    and keep access to the milk
  • 2:10 - 2:13
    and therefore to push
    in a random direction.
  • 2:13 - 2:17
    So it's all about finding
    the simple rules
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    from which complexity emerges.
  • 2:20 - 2:22
    I call this "simplifying complexity".
  • 2:22 - 2:25
    And that is what we do
    at the Chair of Systems Design
  • 2:25 - 2:27
    at ETH Zurich.
  • 2:27 - 2:31
    We collect data on animal populations,
  • 2:31 - 2:34
    analyze complex patterns,
    try to explain them.
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    It requires physicists
    who work with biologists,
  • 2:37 - 2:40
    with mathematicians
    and computer scientists
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    and it is their interaction
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    that produces cross-boundary competence
  • 2:44 - 2:46
    to solve these problems.
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    So again, the whole is greater
    than the sum of the parts.
  • 2:49 - 2:54
    In a way, collaboration is another
    example of a complex system.
  • 2:54 - 2:58
    And you may be asking yourself
    which side I'm on.
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    Biology or Physics?
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    In fact, it's a little different.
  • 3:02 - 3:06
    To explain, I need to tell you
    a short story about myself.
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    When I was a child,
    I loved to build stuff,
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    to create complicated machines.
  • 3:12 - 3:16
    So I set out to study electrical
    engineering and robotics.
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    And my end-of-studies project
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    was about building a robot called ER1 --
  • 3:21 - 3:23
    which looked like this --
  • 3:23 - 3:25
    that would collect information
    from its environment
  • 3:25 - 3:29
    and proceed to follow
    a white line on the ground.
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    It was very, very complicated,
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    but it worked beautifully
    in our test room.
  • 3:34 - 3:38
    And on demo day, professors had assembled
    to grade the projects,
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    so we took ER1 to the evaluation room.
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    It turned out that the light in that room
  • 3:43 - 3:45
    was slightly different.
  • 3:45 - 3:47
    The robot's vision system got confused.
  • 3:47 - 3:49
    At the first bend of the line,
  • 3:49 - 3:53
    it left its course
    and crashed into a wall.
  • 3:53 - 3:56
    We had spent weeks building it
    and all it took to destroy it
  • 3:56 - 4:01
    was a subtle change in the color
    of the light in the room.
  • 4:01 - 4:02
    That's when I realized that
  • 4:02 - 4:04
    the more complicated
    you make a machine
  • 4:04 - 4:06
    the more likely it will fail
  • 4:06 - 4:09
    due to something absolutely unexpected.
  • 4:09 - 4:11
    And I decided that in fact
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    I did not really want
    to create complicated stuff.
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    I wanted to understand complexity,
  • 4:17 - 4:18
    the complexity of the world around us
  • 4:18 - 4:21
    and especially in the animal kingdom,
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    which brings us to bats.
  • 4:24 - 4:27
    Bechstein's bats are a common
    species of European bats.
  • 4:27 - 4:29
    They are very social animals.
  • 4:29 - 4:32
    Mostly, they roost -- or sleep -- together.
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    And they live in maternity colonies,
    which means that, every spring,
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    the females meet
    after the winter hibernation
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    and they stay together
    for about 6 months
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    to rear their young.
  • 4:43 - 4:46
    And they all carry a very small chip,
  • 4:46 - 4:48
    which means that every time
    one of them
  • 4:48 - 4:51
    enters one of these
    specially equipped bat boxes,
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    we know where she is.
  • 4:53 - 4:56
    And more importantly,
    we know with whom she is.
  • 4:56 - 5:00
    So I study roosting associations in bats.
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    And this is what it looks like.
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    During the day, the bats roost
  • 5:05 - 5:07
    in a number of subgroups
    in different boxes.
  • 5:07 - 5:12
    It could be that, on one day,
    the colony is split between two boxes.
  • 5:12 - 5:15
    But, on another day, it could be
    together in a single box
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    or split between 3 or more boxes.
  • 5:17 - 5:20
    And that all seems rather erratic, really,
  • 5:20 - 5:23
    it's called fission-fusion dynamics --
  • 5:23 - 5:27
    -- the property for an animal group
    of regularly splitting
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    and merging into different subgroups.
  • 5:29 - 5:32
    So what we do is to take all these data
  • 5:32 - 5:33
    from all these different days
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    and pool them together
  • 5:35 - 5:38
    to extract a long-term
    association pattern
  • 5:38 - 5:40
    by applying techniques
    of network analysis
  • 5:40 - 5:41
    to get a complete picture
  • 5:41 - 5:44
    of the social structure of the colony.
  • 5:44 - 5:48
    OK? So that's what
    this picture looks like.
  • 5:48 - 5:53
    In this network, all the circles
    are nodes -- individual bats --
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    and the lines between them
    are social bonds --
  • 5:56 - 5:58
    associations between individuals.
  • 5:58 - 6:02
    It turns out this is
    a very interesting picture.
  • 6:02 - 6:05
    This bat colony is organized
    in two different communities
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    which cannot be predicted
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    from the daily fission-fusion dynamics.
  • 6:09 - 6:13
    We call them "cryptic" social units.
  • 6:13 - 6:17
    Even more interesting, in fact:
    every year around October,
  • 6:17 - 6:21
    the colony splits up and all bats
    hibernate separately.
  • 6:21 - 6:22
    But year after year,
  • 6:22 - 6:25
    when the bats come together
    again in the spring,
  • 6:25 - 6:28
    the communities stay the same.
  • 6:28 - 6:33
    So these bats remember their friends
    for a really long time.
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    With a brain of the size of a peanut,
  • 6:35 - 6:40
    they maintain individualized
    long-term social bonds.
  • 6:40 - 6:41
    We didn't know that was possible.
  • 6:41 - 6:45
    We knew that primates and elephants
    and dolphins could do that
  • 6:45 - 6:48
    but compared to bats
    they have huge brains.
  • 6:48 - 6:52
    So, how could it be that the bats
  • 6:52 - 6:54
    maintain this complex
    stable social structure
  • 6:54 - 6:58
    with such limited cognitive abilities?
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    And this is where complexity
    brings an answer.
  • 7:01 - 7:03
    To understand this system,
  • 7:03 - 7:05
    we built a computer model of roosting,
  • 7:05 - 7:07
    based on simple individual rules,
  • 7:07 - 7:10
    and simulated thousands
    and thousands of days
  • 7:10 - 7:12
    in a virtual bat colony.
  • 7:12 - 7:14
    It's a mathematical model,
  • 7:14 - 7:16
    but it is not complicated.
  • 7:16 - 7:19
    What the model told us
    is that, in a nutshell,
  • 7:19 - 7:23
    each bat knows a few other
    colony members as her friends,
  • 7:23 - 7:27
    and is just slightly more likely
    to roost in a box with them.
  • 7:27 - 7:30
    Simple, individual rules.
  • 7:30 - 7:31
    This is all it takes to explain
  • 7:31 - 7:34
    the social complexity of these bats.
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    But it gets better.
  • 7:36 - 7:38
    Between 2010 and 2011,
  • 7:38 - 7:42
    the colony lost more than
    two thirds of its members,
  • 7:42 - 7:45
    probably due to the very cold winter.
  • 7:45 - 7:49
    The next spring, it didn't form
    2 communities like every year,
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    which may have led
    the whole colony to die
  • 7:51 - 7:54
    because it had become too small.
  • 7:54 - 7:59
    Instead, it formed a single
    cohesive social unit,
  • 7:59 - 8:02
    which allowed the colony
    to survive that season
  • 8:02 - 8:05
    and thrive again in the next two years.
  • 8:05 - 8:08
    What we know is that the bats
    are not aware
  • 8:08 - 8:09
    that their colony is doing this.
  • 8:09 - 8:13
    All they do is follow
    simple association rules
  • 8:13 - 8:16
    and from this simplicity
    emerges social complexity,
  • 8:16 - 8:19
    which allows the colony to be resilient
  • 8:19 - 8:23
    against dramatic changes
    in the population structure.
  • 8:23 - 8:25
    And I find this incredible.
  • 8:25 - 8:27
    Now I want to tell you another story.
  • 8:27 - 8:29
    But for this, we have to travel
    from Europe
  • 8:29 - 8:32
    to the Kalahari Desert, in South Africa.
  • 8:32 - 8:34
    This is where meerkats live.
  • 8:34 - 8:38
    I am sure you know meerkats,
    they are fascinating creatures.
  • 8:38 - 8:41
    They live in groups
    with a very strict social hierarchy.
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    There is one dominant pair
    and many subordinates,
  • 8:43 - 8:47
    some acting as sentinels,
    some acting as babysitters,
  • 8:47 - 8:48
    some teaching pups and so on.
  • 8:48 - 8:53
    What we do is put very small GPS collars
    on these animals
  • 8:53 - 8:55
    to study how they move together
  • 8:55 - 8:59
    and what this has to do
    with their social structure.
  • 8:59 - 9:00
    And there is a very interesting example
  • 9:00 - 9:03
    of collective movement in meerkats.
  • 9:03 - 9:04
    In the middle of the reserve,
  • 9:04 - 9:06
    which they live in, lies a road.
  • 9:06 - 9:10
    On this road there are cars,
    so it is dangerous.
  • 9:10 - 9:12
    But the meerkats have to cross it
  • 9:12 - 9:15
    to get from one feeding place to another.
  • 9:15 - 9:19
    So we asked, how exactly
    do they do this?
  • 9:19 - 9:22
    We found out that
    the dominant female
  • 9:22 - 9:24
    is mostly the one who leads
    the group to the road,
  • 9:24 - 9:27
    but when it comes to crossing it,
    crossing the road,
  • 9:27 - 9:29
    she gives way to the subordinates,
  • 9:29 - 9:33
    a manner of saying, you know,
    "Go ahead, tell me if it's safe!".
  • 9:33 - 9:34
    (Laughter)
  • 9:34 - 9:36
    What I didn't know, in fact,
  • 9:36 - 9:39
    was what rules in their behavior
    the meerkats follow
  • 9:39 - 9:42
    for this change at the edge
    of the group to happen
  • 9:42 - 9:46
    and these simple rules were
    sufficient to explain it.
  • 9:46 - 9:47
    So I built a model,
  • 9:47 - 9:52
    a model of simulated meerkats
    crossing a simulated road.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    It's a simplistic model.
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    Moving meerkats are like
    random particles
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    whose unique rule is
    one of alignment.
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    They simply move together.
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    When these particles get to the road,
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    they sense some kind of obstacle
  • 10:06 - 10:08
    and they bounce against it.
  • 10:08 - 10:11
    The only difference between
    the dominant female -- here in red --
  • 10:11 - 10:13
    and the other individuals
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    is that, for her, the height
    of the obstacle,
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    which is in fact the risk
    perceived from the road,
  • 10:18 - 10:20
    is just slightly higher.
  • 10:20 - 10:23
    And this tiny difference
    in the individual rule of movement
  • 10:23 - 10:26
    is sufficient to explain what we observe,
  • 10:26 - 10:30
    that the dominant female
    leads her group to the road
  • 10:30 - 10:34
    and then gives way to the others
    for them to cross first.
  • 10:34 - 10:39
    George Box, who was
    an English statistician, once wrote:
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    "All models are false,
    but some models are useful".
  • 10:43 - 10:46
    And in fact, this model
    is obviously false,
  • 10:46 - 10:50
    because in reality meerkats
    are anything but random particles.
  • 10:50 - 10:52
    But it's also useful, because it tells us
  • 10:52 - 10:58
    that extreme simplicity in movement rules
    at the individual level
  • 10:58 - 11:00
    can result in a great deal of complexity
  • 11:00 - 11:02
    at the level of the group.
  • 11:02 - 11:06
    So again, that's simplifying complexity.
  • 11:06 - 11:08
    And I would like to conclude
    on what this means
  • 11:08 - 11:10
    for the whole species.
  • 11:10 - 11:14
    When the dominant female
    gives way to a subordinate,
  • 11:14 - 11:16
    it's not out of courtesy.
  • 11:16 - 11:18
    In fact, the dominant female
    is extremely important
  • 11:18 - 11:20
    for the cohesion of the group.
  • 11:20 - 11:23
    If she dies on the road,
    the whole group is at risk.
  • 11:23 - 11:26
    So this behavior of risk avoidance
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    is a very old evolutionary response.
  • 11:28 - 11:32
    These meerkats are replicating
    an evolved tactic
  • 11:32 - 11:34
    that is thousands of generations old,
  • 11:34 - 11:37
    and they are adapting it
    to a modern risk;
  • 11:37 - 11:40
    in this case, a road built by humans.
  • 11:40 - 11:43
    They adapt very simple rules
  • 11:43 - 11:45
    and the resulting complex behavior
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    allows them to resist
    human encroachment
  • 11:48 - 11:50
    into their natural habitat.
  • 11:50 - 11:55
    In the end, it may be bats
    who change their social structure
  • 11:55 - 11:57
    in response to a population crash.
  • 11:57 - 12:02
    Or it may be meerkats who show
    a novel adaptation to a human road.
  • 12:02 - 12:04
    Or it may be another species.
  • 12:04 - 12:07
    My message here --
    and it's not a complicated one --
  • 12:07 - 12:10
    but a simple one of wonder and hope.
  • 12:10 - 12:15
    My message here is that animals
    show extraordinary social complexity
  • 12:15 - 12:20
    and this allows them to adapt
    and respond to changes
  • 12:20 - 12:21
    in their environment.
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    In three words: in the animal kingdom,
  • 12:24 - 12:29
    simplicity leads to complexity,
    which leads to resilience.
  • 12:29 - 12:30
    Thank you.
  • 12:30 - 12:33
    (Applause)
Title:
The secret social lives of bats: Nicolas Perony at TEDxZurich
Description:

"Simplicity leads to complexity which leads to resilience," says Nicolas Perony, an engineer turned into a complex systems scientist who studies the social structure of animal groups. Perony shares his findings on the resilience of bat social networks and the adaptation of Kalahari meerkats to human encroachment, and gives us a peek into the wonderful hidden complexity of the animal kingdom.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
12:45

English subtitles

Revisions