Return to Video

A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory | Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu | TEDxBoston

  • 0:08 - 0:10
    Steve Ramirez:
    My first year of grad school,
  • 0:10 - 0:12
    I found myself in my bedroom
  • 0:12 - 0:14
    eating lots of Ben & Jerry's
  • 0:14 - 0:16
    watching some trashy TV
  • 0:16 - 0:19
    and maybe, maybe listening
    to Taylor Swift.
  • 0:19 - 0:21
    I had just gone through a breakup.
  • 0:21 - 0:22
    (Laughter)
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    So for the longest time, all I would do
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    is recall the memory of this
    person over and over again,
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    wishing that I could get
    rid of that gut-wrenching,
  • 0:31 - 0:33
    visceral "blah" feeling.
  • 0:33 - 0:36
    Now, as it turns out,
    I'm a neuroscientist,
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    so I knew that the memory of that person
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    and the awful, emotional undertones
    that color in that memory,
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    are largely mediated
    by separate brain systems.
  • 0:44 - 0:46
    And so I thought, what if we could
    go into the brain
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    and edit out that nauseating feeling
  • 0:48 - 0:51
    but while keeping the memory
    of that person intact?
  • 0:51 - 0:54
    Then I realized, maybe
    that's a little bit lofty for now.
  • 0:54 - 0:56
    So what if we could start off
    by going into the brain
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    and just finding a single
    memory to begin with?
  • 0:59 - 1:02
    Could we jump-start
    that memory back to life,
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    maybe even play with the contents
    of that memory?
  • 1:05 - 1:08
    All that said, there is one person
    in the entire world right now
  • 1:09 - 1:11
    that I really hope is not
    watching this talk.
  • 1:11 - 1:14
    (Laughter)
  • 1:14 - 1:17
    So, there is a catch.
    There is a catch.
  • 1:17 - 1:20
    These ideas probably remind
    you of "Total Recall,"
  • 1:20 - 1:22
    "Eternal Sunshine of the Spotless Mind,"
  • 1:22 - 1:23
    or of "Inception."
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    But the movie stars that we work with
  • 1:25 - 1:27
    are the celebrities of the lab.
  • 1:27 - 1:28
    Xu Liu: Test mice.
  • 1:28 - 1:30
    (Laughter)
  • 1:30 - 1:33
    As neuroscientists,
    we work in the lab with mice
  • 1:33 - 1:36
    trying to understand how memory works.
  • 1:36 - 1:39
    And today, we hope
    to convince you that now
  • 1:39 - 1:42
    we are actually able to activate
    a memory in the brain
  • 1:42 - 1:44
    at the speed of light.
  • 1:44 - 1:47
    To do this, there's only two simple
    steps to follow.
  • 1:47 - 1:51
    First, you find and label
    a memory in the brain,
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    and then you activate it with a switch.
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    As simple as that.
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    (Laughter)
  • 1:58 - 1:59
    SR: Are you convinced?
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    So, turns out finding a memory
    in the brain isn't all that easy.
  • 2:03 - 2:06
    XL: Indeed. This is way more
    difficult than, let's say,
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    finding a needle in a haystack,
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    because at least, you know,
    the needle is still something
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    you can physically put your fingers on.
  • 2:13 - 2:15
    But memory is not.
  • 2:15 - 2:18
    And also, there's way
    more cells in your brain
  • 2:19 - 2:24
    than the number of straws
    in a typical haystack.
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    So yeah, this task does
    seem to be daunting.
  • 2:26 - 2:30
    But luckily, we got help
    from the brain itself.
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    It turned out that all we need
    to do is basically
  • 2:33 - 2:35
    to let the brain form a memory,
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    and then the brain will tell us
    which cells are involved
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    in that particular memory.
  • 2:40 - 2:43
    SR: So what was going on in my brain
  • 2:43 - 2:45
    while I was recalling the memory of an ex?
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    If you were to just completely
    ignore human ethics for a second
  • 2:48 - 2:49
    and slice up my brain right now,
  • 2:49 - 2:51
    you would see that there
    was an amazing number
  • 2:51 - 2:54
    of brain regions that were active
    while recalling that memory.
  • 2:54 - 2:57
    Now one brain region
    that would be robustly active
  • 2:57 - 2:59
    in particular is called the hippocampus,
  • 2:59 - 3:01
    which for decades
    has been implicated in processing
  • 3:01 - 3:04
    the kinds of memories
    that we hold near and dear,
  • 3:04 - 3:06
    which also makes it
    an ideal target to go into
  • 3:06 - 3:09
    and to try and find and maybe
    reactivate a memory.
  • 3:09 - 3:11
    XL: When you zoom in into the hippocampus,
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    of course you will see lots of cells,
  • 3:14 - 3:17
    but we are able to find
    which cells are involved
  • 3:17 - 3:18
    in a particular memory,
  • 3:18 - 3:21
    because whenever a cell is active,
  • 3:21 - 3:22
    like when it's forming a memory,
  • 3:22 - 3:26
    it will also leave a footprint
    that will later allow us to know
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    these cells are recently active.
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    SR: So the same way
    that building lights at night
  • 3:31 - 3:35
    let you know that somebody's probably
    working there at any given moment,
  • 3:35 - 3:37
    in a very real sense, there
    are biological sensors
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    within a cell that are turned on
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    only when that cell was just working.
  • 3:41 - 3:44
    They're sort of biological
    windows that light up
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    to let us know that that cell
    was just active.
  • 3:46 - 3:48
    XL: So we clipped part of this sensor,
  • 3:48 - 3:51
    and attached that to a switch
    to control the cells,
  • 3:51 - 3:55
    and we packed this switch
    into an engineered virus
  • 3:55 - 3:58
    and injected that into the brain
    of the mice.
  • 3:58 - 4:00
    So whenever a memory is being formed,
  • 4:00 - 4:03
    any active cells for that memory
  • 4:03 - 4:05
    will also have this switch installed.
  • 4:05 - 4:08
    SR: So here is what the hippocampus
    looks like
  • 4:08 - 4:10
    after forming a fear memory, for example.
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    The sea of blue that you see here
  • 4:12 - 4:14
    are densely packed brain cells,
  • 4:14 - 4:15
    but the green brain cells,
  • 4:15 - 4:18
    the green brain cells
    are the ones that are holding on
  • 4:18 - 4:19
    to a specific fear memory.
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    So you are looking at the crystallization
  • 4:21 - 4:24
    of the fleeting formation of fear.
  • 4:24 - 4:26
    You're actually looking
    at the cross-section
  • 4:26 - 4:27
    of a memory right now.
  • 4:27 - 4:30
    XL: Now, for the switch
    we have been talking about,
  • 4:30 - 4:33
    ideally, the switch
    has to act really fast.
  • 4:33 - 4:35
    It shouldn't take minutes
    or hours to work.
  • 4:35 - 4:39
    It should act at the speed
    of the brain, in milliseconds.
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    SR: So what do you think, Xu?
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    Could we use, let's say,
    pharmacological drugs
  • 4:44 - 4:45
    to activate or inactivate brain cells?
  • 4:45 - 4:49
    XL: Nah. Drugs are pretty messy.
    They spread everywhere.
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    And also it takes them
    forever to act on cells.
  • 4:52 - 4:56
    So it will not allow us
    to control a memory in real time.
  • 4:56 - 5:00
    So Steve, how about let's zap
    the brain with electricity?
  • 5:00 - 5:03
    SR: So electricity is pretty fast,
  • 5:03 - 5:05
    but we probably wouldn't
    be able to target it
  • 5:05 - 5:07
    to just the specific cells
    that hold onto a memory,
  • 5:07 - 5:09
    and we'd probably fry the brain.
  • 5:09 - 5:12
    XL: Oh. That's true.
    So it looks like, hmm,
  • 5:12 - 5:14
    indeed we need to find a better way
  • 5:14 - 5:18
    to impact the brain at the speed of light.
  • 5:18 - 5:23
    SR: So it just so happens that light
    travels at the speed of light.
  • 5:23 - 5:26
    So maybe we could activate
    or inactivate memories
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    by just using light -
  • 5:28 - 5:29
    XL: That's pretty fast.
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    SR: - and because normally brain cells
  • 5:31 - 5:33
    don't respond to pulses of light,
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    so those that would respond
    to pulses of light
  • 5:35 - 5:37
    are those that contain
    a light-sensitive switch.
  • 5:37 - 5:39
    Now to do that, first we need
    to trick brain cells
  • 5:39 - 5:41
    to respond to laser beams.
  • 5:41 - 5:42
    XL: Yep. You heard it right.
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    We are trying to shoot lasers
    into the brain.
  • 5:44 - 5:45
    (Laughter)
  • 5:45 - 5:49
    SR: And the technique that lets us do that
    is optogenetics.
  • 5:49 - 5:52
    Optogenetics gave us this
    light switch that we can use
  • 5:52 - 5:54
    to turn brain cells on or off,
  • 5:54 - 5:56
    and the name of that switch
    is channelrhodopsin,
  • 5:56 - 5:59
    seen here as these green dots
    attached to this brain cell.
  • 5:59 - 6:02
    You can think of channelrhodopsin
    as a sort of light-sensitive switch
  • 6:02 - 6:04
    that can be artificially
    installed in brain cells
  • 6:05 - 6:06
    so that now we can use that switch
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    to activate or inactivate the brain
    cell simply by clicking it,
  • 6:09 - 6:12
    and in this case we click it on
    with pulses of light.
  • 6:12 - 6:16
    XL: So we attach this light-sensitive
    switch of channelrhodopsin
  • 6:16 - 6:18
    to the sensor we've been talking about
  • 6:18 - 6:20
    and inject this into the brain.
  • 6:20 - 6:24
    So whenever a memory is being formed,
  • 6:24 - 6:26
    any active cell for that particular memory
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    will also have this light-sensitive switch
    installed in it
  • 6:29 - 6:32
    so that we can control these cells
  • 6:32 - 6:36
    by the flipping of a laser
    just like this one you see.
  • 6:36 - 6:39
    SR: So let's put all of this
    to the test now.
  • 6:39 - 6:41
    What we can do is we can take our mice
  • 6:41 - 6:44
    and then we can put them in a box
    that looks exactly like this one,
  • 6:44 - 6:46
    and then we can give them
    a very mild foot shock
  • 6:46 - 6:49
    so that they form a fear
    memory of this box.
  • 6:49 - 6:51
    They learn that something
    bad happened here.
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    Now with our system,
    the cells that are active
  • 6:53 - 6:55
    in the hippocampus
    in the making of this memory,
  • 6:55 - 6:58
    only those cells will now
    contain channelrhodopsin.
  • 6:58 - 7:01
    XL: When you are as small as a mouse,
  • 7:01 - 7:05
    it feels as if the whole
    world is trying to get you.
  • 7:05 - 7:07
    So your best response of defense
  • 7:07 - 7:09
    is trying to be undetected.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    Whenever a mouse is in fear,
  • 7:11 - 7:13
    it will show this very typical behavior
  • 7:13 - 7:15
    by staying at one corner of the box,
  • 7:15 - 7:18
    trying to not move any part of its body,
  • 7:18 - 7:21
    and this posture is called freezing.
  • 7:21 - 7:25
    So if a mouse remembers that something
    bad happened in this box,
  • 7:25 - 7:28
    and when we put them
    back into the same box,
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    it will basically show freezing
  • 7:30 - 7:32
    because it doesn't want to be detected
  • 7:32 - 7:35
    by any potential threats in this box.
  • 7:35 - 7:36
    SR: So you can think of freezing as,
  • 7:36 - 7:39
    you're walking down the street
    minding your own business,
  • 7:39 - 7:41
    and then out of nowhere
    you almost run into
  • 7:41 - 7:43
    an ex-girlfriend or ex-boyfriend,
  • 7:43 - 7:44
    and now those terrifying two seconds
  • 7:44 - 7:47
    where you start thinking,
    "What do I do? Do I say hi?
  • 7:47 - 7:49
    Do I shake their hand?
    Do I turn around and run away?
  • 7:49 - 7:51
    Do I sit here and pretend
    like I don't exist?"
  • 7:51 - 7:55
    Those kinds of fleeting thoughts
    that physically incapacitate you,
  • 7:55 - 7:57
    that temporarily give you
    that deer-in-headlights look.
  • 7:57 - 8:00
    XL: However, if you put the mouse
    in a completely different new box,
  • 8:00 - 8:02
    like the next one,
  • 8:02 - 8:04
    it will not be afraid of this box
  • 8:04 - 8:09
    because there's no reason that it
    will be afraid of this new environment.
  • 8:09 - 8:12
    But what if we put
    the mouse in this new box
  • 8:12 - 8:16
    but at the same time,
    we activate the fear memory
  • 8:16 - 8:18
    using lasers just like we did before?
  • 8:18 - 8:21
    Are we going to bring back the fear memory
  • 8:21 - 8:25
    for the first box into this
    completely new environment?
  • 8:25 - 8:28
    SR: All right,
    and here's the million-dollar experiment.
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    Now to bring back to life
    the memory of that day,
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    I remember that the Red Sox had just won,
  • 8:33 - 8:35
    it was a green spring day,
  • 8:35 - 8:37
    perfect for going up and down the river
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    and then maybe going to the North End
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    to get some cannolis, #justsaying.
  • 8:41 - 8:44
    Now Xu and I, on the other hand,
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    were in a completely windowless black room
  • 8:47 - 8:51
    not making any ocular movement
    that even remotely resembles an eye blink
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    because our eyes were fixed
    onto a computer screen.
  • 8:53 - 8:56
    We were looking at this mouse
    here trying to activate a memory
  • 8:56 - 8:58
    for the first time using our technique.
  • 8:58 - 9:00
    XL: And this is what we saw.
  • 9:00 - 9:03
    When we first put the mouse into this box,
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    it's exploring, sniffing
    around, walking around,
  • 9:06 - 9:07
    minding its own business,
  • 9:07 - 9:09
    because actually by nature,
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    mice are pretty curious animals.
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    They want to know,
    what's going on in this new box?
  • 9:14 - 9:15
    It's interesting.
  • 9:15 - 9:19
    But the moment we turned on the laser,
    like you see now,
  • 9:19 - 9:22
    all of a sudden the mouse
    entered this freezing mode.
  • 9:22 - 9:26
    It stayed here and tried not to move
    any part of its body.
  • 9:26 - 9:28
    Clearly it's freezing.
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    So indeed, it looks like
    we are able to bring back
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    the fear memory for the first box
  • 9:32 - 9:36
    in this completely new environment.
  • 9:36 - 9:38
    While watching this, Steve and I
  • 9:38 - 9:40
    are as shocked as the mouse itself.
  • 9:40 - 9:41
    (Laughter)
  • 9:41 - 9:45
    So after the experiment,
    the two of us just left the room
  • 9:45 - 9:46
    without saying anything.
  • 9:46 - 9:50
    After a kind of long,
    awkward period of time,
  • 9:50 - 9:52
    Steve broke the silence.
  • 9:52 - 9:54
    SR: "Did that just work?"
  • 9:54 - 9:57
    XL: "Yes," I said. "Indeed it worked!"
  • 9:57 - 9:59
    We're really excited about this.
  • 9:59 - 10:02
    And then we published our findings
  • 10:02 - 10:04
    in the journal Nature.
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    Ever since the publication of our work,
  • 10:06 - 10:09
    we've been receiving numerous comments
  • 10:09 - 10:11
    from all over the Internet.
  • 10:11 - 10:14
    Maybe we can take a look at some of those.
  • 10:17 - 10:20
    SR: So the first thing
    that you'll notice is that people have
  • 10:20 - 10:22
    really strong opinions
    about this kind of work.
  • 10:22 - 10:24
    Now I happen to completely
    agree with the optimism
  • 10:24 - 10:25
    of this first quote,
  • 10:25 - 10:28
    because on a scale
    of zero to Morgan Freeman's voice,
  • 10:28 - 10:30
    it happens to be
    one of the most evocative accolades
  • 10:30 - 10:32
    that I've heard come our way.
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    (Laughter)
  • 10:34 - 10:37
    But as you'll see, it's not
    the only opinion that's out there.
  • 10:37 - 10:40
    XL: Indeed, if we take
    a look at the second one,
  • 10:40 - 10:42
    I think we can all agree that it's, meh,
  • 10:42 - 10:44
    probably not as positive.
  • 10:44 - 10:46
    But this also reminds us that,
  • 10:46 - 10:48
    although we are still working with mice,
  • 10:48 - 10:52
    it's probably a good idea
    to start thinking and discussing
  • 10:52 - 10:55
    about the possible ethical ramifications
  • 10:55 - 10:57
    of memory control.
  • 10:57 - 10:59
    SR: Now, in the spirit of the third quote,
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    we want to tell you about a recent project
  • 11:01 - 11:04
    that we've been working on in lab
    that we've called Project Inception.
  • 11:07 - 11:10
    So we reasoned that
    now that we can reactivate a memory,
  • 11:10 - 11:13
    what if we do so but then
    begin to tinker with that memory?
  • 11:13 - 11:16
    Could we possibly even
    turn it into a false memory?
  • 11:16 - 11:20
    XL: So all memory
    is sophisticated and dynamic,
  • 11:20 - 11:23
    but if just for simplicity,
    let's imagine memory
  • 11:23 - 11:25
    as a movie clip.
  • 11:25 - 11:27
    So far what we've told you
    is basically we can control
  • 11:27 - 11:29
    this "play" button of the clip
  • 11:29 - 11:34
    so that we can play this video clip
    any time, anywhere.
  • 11:34 - 11:36
    But is there a possibility
    that we can actually get
  • 11:36 - 11:39
    inside the brain and edit this movie clip
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    so that we can make it
    different from the original?
  • 11:42 - 11:44
    Yes we can.
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    Turned out that all we need
    to do is basically
  • 11:47 - 11:51
    reactivate a memory
    using lasers just like we did before,
  • 11:51 - 11:54
    but at the same time,
    if we present new information
  • 11:54 - 11:58
    and allow this new information
    to incorporate into this old memory,
  • 11:58 - 12:01
    this will change the memory.
  • 12:01 - 12:04
    It's sort of like making a remix tape.
  • 12:04 - 12:07
    SR: So how do we do this?
  • 12:07 - 12:09
    Rather than finding
    a fear memory in the brain,
  • 12:09 - 12:11
    we can start by taking our animals,
  • 12:11 - 12:14
    and let's say we put them in a blue box
    like this blue box here
  • 12:14 - 12:17
    and we find the brain cells
    that represent that blue box
  • 12:17 - 12:19
    and we trick them to respond
    to pulses of light
  • 12:19 - 12:20
    exactly like we had said before.
  • 12:20 - 12:23
    Now the next day, we can take
    our animals and place them
  • 12:23 - 12:26
    in a red box that they've never
    experienced before.
  • 12:26 - 12:28
    We can shoot light
    into the brain to reactivate
  • 12:28 - 12:29
    the memory of the blue box.
  • 12:29 - 12:31
    So what would happen here if,
  • 12:31 - 12:33
    while the animal is recalling
    the memory of the blue box,
  • 12:33 - 12:36
    we gave it a couple of mild foot shocks?
  • 12:36 - 12:38
    So here we're trying to artificially
    make an association
  • 12:38 - 12:40
    between the memory of the blue box
  • 12:40 - 12:42
    and the foot shocks themselves.
  • 12:42 - 12:43
    We're just trying to connect the two.
  • 12:43 - 12:45
    So to test if we had done so,
  • 12:45 - 12:47
    we can take our animals once again
  • 12:47 - 12:48
    and place them back in the blue box.
  • 12:48 - 12:51
    Again, we had just reactivated
    the memory of the blue box
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    while the animal got a couple
    of mild foot shocks,
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    and now the animal suddenly freezes.
  • 12:56 - 12:59
    It's as though it's recalling being
    mildly shocked in this environment
  • 12:59 - 13:02
    even though that never actually happened.
  • 13:02 - 13:04
    So it formed a false memory,
  • 13:04 - 13:06
    because it's falsely
    fearing an environment
  • 13:06 - 13:07
    where, technically speaking,
  • 13:07 - 13:09
    nothing bad actually happened to it.
  • 13:09 - 13:12
    XL: So, so far we are only talking about
  • 13:12 - 13:14
    this light-controlled "on" switch.
  • 13:14 - 13:17
    In fact, we also have
    a light-controlled "off" switch,
  • 13:17 - 13:19
    and it's very easy to imagine
  • 13:19 - 13:22
    that by installing this
    light-controlled "off" switch,
  • 13:22 - 13:26
    we can also turn off a memory,
    any time, anywhere.
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    So everything
    we've been talking about today
  • 13:30 - 13:34
    is based on this philosophically
    charged principle of neuroscience
  • 13:34 - 13:39
    that the mind, with its seemingly
    mysterious properties,
  • 13:39 - 13:42
    is actually made of physical stuff
    that we can tinker with.
  • 13:42 - 13:44
    SR: And for me personally,
  • 13:44 - 13:45
    I see a world where we can reactivate
  • 13:45 - 13:47
    any kind of memory that we'd like.
  • 13:47 - 13:51
    I also see a world where we can
    erase unwanted memories.
  • 13:51 - 13:53
    Now, I even see a world
    where editing memories
  • 13:53 - 13:54
    is something of a reality,
  • 13:54 - 13:56
    because we're living in a time
    where it's possible
  • 13:56 - 13:59
    to pluck questions from the tree
    of science fiction
  • 13:59 - 14:01
    and to ground them
    in experimental reality.
  • 14:01 - 14:02
    XL: Nowadays, people in the lab
  • 14:02 - 14:05
    and people in other groups
    all over the world
  • 14:05 - 14:09
    are using similar methods
    to activate or edit memories,
  • 14:09 - 14:12
    whether that's old or new,
    positive or negative,
  • 14:12 - 14:15
    all sorts of memories
    so that we can understand
  • 14:15 - 14:17
    how memory works.
  • 14:17 - 14:19
    SR: For example, one group in our lab
  • 14:19 - 14:21
    was able to find the brain cells
    that make up a fear memory
  • 14:22 - 14:24
    and converted them into
    a pleasurable memory, just like that.
  • 14:24 - 14:28
    That's exactly what I mean about editing
    these kinds of processes.
  • 14:28 - 14:30
    Now one dude in lab
    was even able to reactivate
  • 14:30 - 14:32
    memories of female mice in male mice,
  • 14:32 - 14:34
    which rumor has it
    is a pleasurable experience.
  • 14:34 - 14:39
    XL: Indeed, we are living
    in a very exciting moment
  • 14:39 - 14:42
    where science doesn't have
    any arbitrary speed limits
  • 14:42 - 14:46
    but is only bound by our own imagination.
  • 14:46 - 14:48
    SR: And finally,
    what do we make of all this?
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    How do we push this technology forward?
  • 14:50 - 14:52
    These are the questions
    that should not remain
  • 14:52 - 14:53
    just inside the lab,
  • 14:53 - 14:56
    and so one goal of today's talk
    was to bring everybody
  • 14:56 - 14:58
    up to speed with the kind
    of stuff that's possible
  • 14:58 - 14:59
    in modern neuroscience,
  • 15:00 - 15:01
    but now, just as importantly,
  • 15:01 - 15:04
    to actively engage everybody
    in this conversation.
  • 15:04 - 15:07
    So let's think together as a team
    about what this all means
  • 15:07 - 15:09
    and where we can and should go from here,
  • 15:09 - 15:11
    because Xu and I think we all have
  • 15:11 - 15:14
    some really big decisions ahead of us.
  • 15:14 - 15:15
    Thank you.
    XL: Thank you.
  • 15:15 - 15:17
    (Applause)
Title:
A mouse. A laser beam. A manipulated memory | Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu | TEDxBoston
Description:

Can we edit the content of our memories? It’s a sci-fi-tinged question that Steve Ramirez and Xu Liu are asking in their lab at MIT. Essentially, the pair shoot a laser beam into the brain of a living mouse to activate and manipulate its memory. In this unexpectedly amusing talk they share not only how, but -- more importantly -- why they do this.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
15:25

English subtitles

Revisions