Return to Video

How to change your behavior for the better

  • 0:01 - 0:02
    Hi.
  • 0:03 - 0:05
    You might have noticed
    that I have half a beard.
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    It's not because I lost a bet.
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    Many years ago, I was badly burned.
  • 0:11 - 0:13
    Most of my body is covered with scars,
  • 0:13 - 0:15
    including the right side of my face.
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    I just don't have hair.
    That's just how it happened.
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    It looks symmetrical, but almost.
  • 0:20 - 0:23
    Anyway, now that we discussed facial hair,
  • 0:23 - 0:26
    let's move to social science.
  • 0:26 - 0:32
    And in particular, I want us to think
    about where is the potential for humanity
  • 0:32 - 0:33
    and where we are now.
  • 0:34 - 0:36
    And if you think about it,
    there's a big gap
  • 0:36 - 0:39
    between where we think we could be
    and where we are,
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    and it's in all kinds of areas.
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    So let me ask you:
  • 0:43 - 0:47
    How many of you in the last month
    have eaten more than you think you should?
  • 0:47 - 0:49
    Just kind of general. OK.
  • 0:49 - 0:53
    How many of you in the last month have
    exercised less than you think you should?
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    OK, and for how many of you
    has raising your hands twice
  • 0:56 - 0:58
    been the most exercise you got today?
  • 0:58 - 1:01
    (Laughter)
  • 1:02 - 1:05
    How many of you have ever
    texted while driving?
  • 1:06 - 1:10
    OK, we're getting honest.
    Let's test your honesty.
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    How many people here in the last month
  • 1:12 - 1:14
    have not always washed your hands
    when you left the bathroom?
  • 1:15 - 1:16
    (Laughter)
  • 1:16 - 1:17
    A little less honest.
  • 1:17 - 1:21
    By the way, it's interesting how we're
    willing to admit texting and driving
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    but not washing our hands,
    that's difficult.
  • 1:23 - 1:25
    (Laughter)
  • 1:25 - 1:27
    We can go on and on.
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    The problem, the topic is
    that there's lots of things
  • 1:29 - 1:32
    when we know what we could do --
  • 1:32 - 1:35
    we could be very, very different
    but we're acting in a very different way.
  • 1:35 - 1:37
    And when we think
    how do we bridge that gap,
  • 1:37 - 1:41
    the usual answer is, "Just tell people."
  • 1:41 - 1:44
    For example, just tell people
    that texting and driving is dangerous.
  • 1:44 - 1:46
    Did you know it's dangerous?
    You should stop doing it.
  • 1:46 - 1:49
    You tell people something
    is dangerous, and they will stop.
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    Texting and driving is one example.
  • 1:51 - 1:53
    Another very sad example
    is that in the US,
  • 1:53 - 1:57
    we spend between seven
    and eight hundred million dollars a year
  • 1:57 - 1:59
    on what's called "financial literacy."
  • 1:59 - 2:02
    And what do we get
    as a consequence of that?
  • 2:02 - 2:06
    There was recently a study that looked
    at all the research ever to be conducted
  • 2:06 - 2:09
    on financial literacy --
    what's called a meta-analysis.
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    And what they found is that
    when you tell people,
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    you teach them financial literacy,
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    they learn and they remember.
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    But do people execute? Not so much.
  • 2:19 - 2:21
    The improvement is about
    three or four percent
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    immediately after the course,
  • 2:23 - 2:25
    and then it goes down.
  • 2:25 - 2:26
    And at the end of the day,
  • 2:26 - 2:30
    the improvement is about 0.1 percent --
  • 2:30 - 2:34
    not zero, but as humanly close
    to zero as possible.
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    (Laughter)
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    So that's the sad news.
  • 2:38 - 2:40
    The sad news is, giving
    information to people
  • 2:40 - 2:43
    is just not a good recipe
    to change behavior.
  • 2:43 - 2:44
    What is?
  • 2:44 - 2:47
    Well, social science
    has made lots of strides,
  • 2:47 - 2:51
    and the basic insight is that
    if we want to change behavior,
  • 2:51 - 2:53
    we have to change the environment.
  • 2:53 - 2:57
    The right way is not to change people,
    it's to change the environment.
  • 2:57 - 3:00
    And I want to present a very simpleminded
    model of how to think about it:
  • 3:00 - 3:02
    it's to think about behavioral change
  • 3:02 - 3:05
    in the same way that we think about
    sending a rocket to space.
  • 3:06 - 3:08
    When we think about
    sending a rocket to space,
  • 3:08 - 3:10
    we want to do two main things.
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    The first one is to reduce friction.
  • 3:12 - 3:15
    We want to take the rocket
    and have as little friction as possible
  • 3:15 - 3:17
    so it's the most aerodynamic possible.
  • 3:17 - 3:21
    And the second thing is we want
    to load as much fuel as possible,
  • 3:21 - 3:25
    to give it the most amount
    of motivation, energy to do its task.
  • 3:25 - 3:28
    And behavior change is the same thing.
  • 3:28 - 3:30
    So let's first talk about friction.
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    In this particular case study
    I'll tell you about,
  • 3:34 - 3:37
    there's a pharmacy, an online pharmacy.
  • 3:37 - 3:39
    Imagine you go to your doctor.
  • 3:39 - 3:41
    You have a long-term illness,
  • 3:41 - 3:44
    your doctor prescribes
    to you a medication,
  • 3:44 - 3:46
    you sign up for this online pharmacy
  • 3:46 - 3:49
    and you get your medication
    in the mail every 90 days.
  • 3:49 - 3:52
    Every 90 days, medication,
    medication, medication.
  • 3:52 - 3:56
    And this online pharmacy
    wants to switch people
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    from branded medication
    to generic medication.
  • 3:59 - 4:01
    So they send people letters, and they say,
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    "Please, please, please,
    switch to generics.
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    You will save money, we will save money,
    your employer will save money."
  • 4:07 - 4:08
    And what do people do?
  • 4:09 - 4:10
    Nothing.
  • 4:10 - 4:12
    So they try all kinds of things
    and nothing happens.
  • 4:12 - 4:15
    So for one year, they give people
    an amazing offer.
  • 4:15 - 4:17
    They send people a letter, and they say,
  • 4:17 - 4:21
    "If you switch to generics now,
    it will be free for a whole year."
  • 4:21 - 4:23
    Free for a whole year. Amazing!
  • 4:23 - 4:26
    What percentage of people
    do you think switched?
  • 4:27 - 4:28
    Less than 10 percent.
  • 4:29 - 4:31
    At this point, they show up to my office.
  • 4:31 - 4:33
    And they come to complain.
  • 4:34 - 4:35
    Why did they pick me?
  • 4:35 - 4:38
    I wrote a couple of papers
    on the "allure of free."
  • 4:38 - 4:41
    In those papers, we showed
    that if you reduce the price of something
  • 4:41 - 4:44
    for, let's say, 10 cents to one cent,
    nothing much happens.
  • 4:44 - 4:47
    You reduce it from one cent to zero,
    now people get excited.
  • 4:47 - 4:48
    (Laughter)
  • 4:48 - 4:51
    And they said, "Look, we read these
    papers on 'free,' we gave 'free.'
  • 4:51 - 4:53
    Not working as we expected.
  • 4:53 - 4:54
    What's going on?
  • 4:54 - 4:57
    I said, "You know, maybe
    it's a question of friction."
  • 4:58 - 4:59
    They said, "What do you mean?"
  • 4:59 - 5:01
    I said, "People are starting with branded.
  • 5:01 - 5:03
    They can do nothing and end with branded.
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    To move to generic, they have to choose
    generic over branded,
  • 5:07 - 5:09
    but they also have to do something.
  • 5:09 - 5:11
    They have to return the letter."
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    So this is what we call
    a "confounded design."
  • 5:13 - 5:15
    Two things are happening at the same time.
  • 5:15 - 5:17
    It's branded versus generic,
  • 5:17 - 5:20
    but it's doing nothing
    versus doing something.
  • 5:20 - 5:22
    So I said, "Why don't we switch it?
  • 5:22 - 5:26
    Why don't we send people a letter
    and say, 'We're switching you to generics.
  • 5:26 - 5:28
    You don't need to do anything.
  • 5:28 - 5:31
    If you want to stay with branded,
    please return the letter.'"
  • 5:31 - 5:32
    (Laughter)
  • 5:32 - 5:33
    Right?
  • 5:33 - 5:35
    What do you think happened?
  • 5:37 - 5:38
    Lawyers, lawyers happened.
  • 5:38 - 5:42
    (Laughter)
  • 5:44 - 5:46
    It turns out, this is illegal.
  • 5:46 - 5:49
    (Laughter)
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    By the way, for brainstorming
    and creativity,
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    doing things that are illegal
    and immoral, it's fine,
  • 5:57 - 6:00
    as long as it's just
    in the brainstorming phase.
  • 6:00 - 6:02
    (Laughter)
  • 6:02 - 6:04
    But this was the purity of the idea,
  • 6:04 - 6:08
    because the initial design was,
    the branded had the no-action benefit.
  • 6:08 - 6:12
    In my illegal, immoral design,
    generic had the no-action benefit.
  • 6:12 - 6:15
    But they agreed to give people
    a T-intersection:
  • 6:15 - 6:16
    send people a letter and say,
  • 6:16 - 6:18
    "If you don't return this letter,
  • 6:18 - 6:21
    we will be forced
    to stop your medications.
  • 6:21 - 6:25
    But when you return the letter,
    you could choose branded at this price,
  • 6:25 - 6:26
    generic at this price."
  • 6:27 - 6:29
    Now people had to take an action.
  • 6:29 - 6:31
    They were on even footing. Right?
  • 6:31 - 6:33
    It wasn't that one had
    the no-action benefit.
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    What percentage do you think switched?
  • 6:36 - 6:38
    The vast majority switched.
  • 6:38 - 6:39
    So what does it tell us?
  • 6:39 - 6:42
    Do people like generics,
    or do we like branded?
  • 6:42 - 6:44
    We hate returning letters.
  • 6:44 - 6:46
    (Laughter)
  • 6:46 - 6:51
    This is the story of friction:
    small things really matter.
  • 6:51 - 6:54
    And friction is about taking
    the desired behavior
  • 6:54 - 6:57
    and saying, where do we have
    too much friction
  • 6:57 - 6:59
    so it's slowing people down
    from acting on it?
  • 6:59 - 7:02
    And every time you see
    that the desired behavior
  • 7:02 - 7:03
    and the easy behavior are not aligned,
  • 7:04 - 7:06
    it means we want to try and realign them.
  • 7:07 - 7:09
    That's the first part.
    We talked about friction.
  • 7:09 - 7:11
    Now let's talk about motivation.
  • 7:11 - 7:12
    In this particular study,
  • 7:12 - 7:16
    we were trying to get very poor people
    in a slum called Kibera in Kenya
  • 7:16 - 7:19
    to save a little bit of money
    for a rainy day.
  • 7:19 - 7:22
    You know, if you're very, very poor,
    you have no extra money,
  • 7:22 - 7:23
    you live hand to mouth,
  • 7:23 - 7:26
    and from time to time, bad things happen.
  • 7:26 - 7:30
    And when something bad happens,
    you have nothing to draw on, you borrow.
  • 7:30 - 7:36
    The Kibera people can borrow at sometimes
    up to 10 percent interest a week.
  • 7:36 - 7:39
    And then, of course,
    it's really hard to get out of it.
  • 7:39 - 7:41
    You live hand to mouth,
    something bad happens,
  • 7:41 - 7:44
    you borrow, things get worse
    and worse and worse.
  • 7:44 - 7:47
    So we wanted people to keep
    a little bit of money for a rainy day.
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    And we thought about
    what is the motivation,
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    what is the fuel that we need to add?
  • 7:53 - 7:55
    And we tried all kinds of things.
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    Some people, we texted them
    once a week and said,
  • 7:58 - 8:02
    "Please try to save 100 shillings" --
    about a dollar -- "this week."
  • 8:03 - 8:07
    Some people, we sent a text message
    as if it came from their kids.
  • 8:07 - 8:11
    So it said, "Hi Mom, hi Dad,
    this is little Joey" --
  • 8:11 - 8:13
    whatever the name of the kid was --
  • 8:13 - 8:16
    "Try and save 100 shillings this week
    for the future of our family."
  • 8:17 - 8:20
    Right? I'm Jewish, a little bit
    of guilt always works.
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    (Laughter)
  • 8:22 - 8:23
    Some people got 10 percent.
  • 8:23 - 8:26
    "Save up to a hundred shillings,
    we'll give you 10 percent."
  • 8:26 - 8:28
    Some people got 20 percent.
  • 8:28 - 8:31
    Some people got also
    10 percent and 20 percent,
  • 8:31 - 8:33
    but they got it with loss aversion.
  • 8:33 - 8:34
    What is loss aversion?
  • 8:34 - 8:37
    Loss aversion is the idea
    that we hate losing
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    more than we enjoy gaining.
  • 8:39 - 8:41
    Now, think about somebody
    who is in a 10-percent condition
  • 8:41 - 8:43
    and they put 40 shillings in.
  • 8:43 - 8:46
    They put 40 shillings,
    we give them four more,
  • 8:46 - 8:47
    they say thank you very much.
  • 8:47 - 8:49
    That person gave up six.
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    They could have gotten six more
    if they gave a hundred,
  • 8:52 - 8:53
    but they don't see it.
  • 8:53 - 8:56
    So we created what we call pre-match.
  • 8:56 - 8:58
    We put the 10 shillings in
    at the beginning of the week.
  • 8:58 - 9:00
    We said, "It's waiting for you!"
  • 9:00 - 9:03
    And then if somebody puts 40 in,
    we say, "Oh, you put 40 in,
  • 9:03 - 9:05
    we're leaving four,
    and we're taking six back."
  • 9:05 - 9:08
    So in both cases, pre-match or post-match,
  • 9:08 - 9:10
    people get 10 percent.
  • 9:10 - 9:12
    But in the pre-match,
  • 9:12 - 9:15
    they see the money they did not match
    leaving their account.
  • 9:16 - 9:19
    So we have text, text from kids,
    10 percent, 20 percent,
  • 9:19 - 9:20
    pre-match, post-match.
  • 9:20 - 9:22
    And we had one more condition.
  • 9:22 - 9:25
    It was a coin about this size,
  • 9:25 - 9:27
    with 24 numbers written on it.
  • 9:27 - 9:31
    And we asked them to put the coin
    somewhere in their hut,
  • 9:31 - 9:34
    and every week, take a knife
    and scratch the number for that week --
  • 9:34 - 9:36
    week one, two, three, four --
  • 9:36 - 9:39
    scratch it like a minus
    if they didn't save
  • 9:39 - 9:41
    and scratch it up and down if they saved.
  • 9:42 - 9:43
    Now, think to yourself:
  • 9:43 - 9:46
    Which one of those methods
    do you think worked the best?
  • 9:46 - 9:49
    Text, text from the kids,
    10 percent, 20 percent,
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    beginning of the week,
    end of the week, and the coin?
  • 9:51 - 9:54
    I'll tell you what
    the average people think.
  • 9:54 - 9:56
    We've done these studies of prediction,
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    both in the US and in Kenya.
  • 9:58 - 10:01
    People think that 20 percent
    will get a lot of action,
  • 10:01 - 10:02
    10 percent less,
  • 10:02 - 10:04
    the rest of it will do nothing --
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    kids, coin, doesn't matter.
  • 10:07 - 10:10
    People think loss aversion
    will have a small effect.
  • 10:11 - 10:12
    What actually happened?
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    Sending a text reminder once a week
  • 10:15 - 10:16
    helps a lot.
  • 10:17 - 10:18
    Good news!
  • 10:18 - 10:22
    This program lasted six months.
    People forget. Reminding people is great.
  • 10:22 - 10:25
    Ten percent at the end
    of the week helped some more.
  • 10:25 - 10:26
    Financial incentives work.
  • 10:27 - 10:31
    Twenty percent at the end of the week --
    just like 10 percent, no difference.
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    Ten percent in the beginning of the week
  • 10:34 - 10:35
    helps some more.
  • 10:35 - 10:36
    Loss aversion works.
  • 10:36 - 10:38
    Twenty percent in
    the beginning of the week,
  • 10:38 - 10:41
    just like 10 percent in the beginning
    of the week, no difference.
  • 10:41 - 10:44
    And the text message from the kids
    was just as effective
  • 10:44 - 10:47
    as 20 percent plus loss aversion --
  • 10:47 - 10:49
    which is amazing, right?
  • 10:49 - 10:52
    It's amazing how motivating
    messages from kids were.
  • 10:52 - 10:56
    And one conclusion is,
    we don't use kids enough.
  • 10:56 - 10:58
    (Laughter)
  • 10:58 - 11:03
    And, of course, I don't mean
    in a child labor sense.
  • 11:04 - 11:06
    But if you think about
    parents and their kids,
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    we are the best that we can for our kids,
  • 11:09 - 11:11
    and we think about the future,
  • 11:11 - 11:12
    and I think we should think
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    about how to use that amazing
    source of motivation
  • 11:15 - 11:18
    to get parents to behave in a better way.
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    But the big surprise
    of this study was the coin.
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    The coin basically doubled savings
    compared to everything else.
  • 11:26 - 11:28
    And now the question is: Why?
    What was it about the coin?
  • 11:29 - 11:32
    So I'll tell you how I started
    thinking about the coin,
  • 11:32 - 11:33
    and then we'll come back to it.
  • 11:33 - 11:36
    So you know, when I do research
    on, let's say, buying coffee,
  • 11:36 - 11:39
    I don't need to go anywhere.
    I can sit in my office.
  • 11:39 - 11:41
    I've bought enough coffee.
    I know how it works.
  • 11:41 - 11:43
    The details, I'm familiar with.
  • 11:43 - 11:46
    When you do research in some
    of the poorest places in the world,
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    you have to go and visit
    and see what's going on
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    and get some insight
    about how the system works.
  • 11:52 - 11:53
    And on that particular day,
  • 11:53 - 11:56
    I'm in a place called Soweto
    in South Africa,
  • 11:56 - 11:59
    and I'm sitting in a place
    that sells funeral insurance.
  • 12:00 - 12:04
    You know, in the US people spend
    crazy amounts of money on weddings?
  • 12:04 - 12:06
    In South Africa, it's funerals.
  • 12:06 - 12:11
    People spend up to a year
    or two years of income on funerals.
  • 12:12 - 12:15
    And I sit in this place --
  • 12:15 - 12:20
    by the way, before you judge the South
    Africans as being irrational with this,
  • 12:20 - 12:21
    I just want to remind you
  • 12:21 - 12:24
    that spending a lot of money
    on funerals compared to weddings,
  • 12:24 - 12:27
    at least you know for sure
    you only have one.
  • 12:27 - 12:30
    (Laughter)
  • 12:36 - 12:41
    OK, so I sit in this place
    that sells funeral insurance.
  • 12:41 - 12:45
    And this guy comes in with his son --
    his son is about 12 --
  • 12:45 - 12:48
    and he buys funeral insurance for a week.
  • 12:49 - 12:51
    It will cover 90 percent
    of his funeral expense
  • 12:51 - 12:54
    only if he dies in the next seven days.
  • 12:54 - 12:58
    Right? These are very poor people,
    they buy small amounts of insurance
  • 12:58 - 12:59
    and small amount of soap and such.
  • 12:59 - 13:01
    And he gets that certificate,
  • 13:01 - 13:04
    and in a very ceremonious way,
    he gives it to his son.
  • 13:04 - 13:08
    And as he gives it to his son,
    I think to myself, why the ceremony?
  • 13:08 - 13:09
    What is this father doing?
  • 13:09 - 13:13
    Now, think about the breadwinner
    that decides on that particular day
  • 13:13 - 13:16
    to direct some money
    into insurance or savings.
  • 13:17 - 13:19
    What is the family going to see tonight?
  • 13:20 - 13:22
    They're going to see less.
  • 13:22 - 13:26
    Right? At that level of poverty, there'll
    be less food, less kerosene, less water --
  • 13:26 - 13:27
    something less tonight.
  • 13:27 - 13:31
    And what his father was doing
    and what our coin was trying to do
  • 13:31 - 13:33
    is to say, yes, there's less
    food on the table,
  • 13:33 - 13:35
    but there's another activity.
  • 13:36 - 13:40
    You see, what happened is, there are
    many good, important economic activities,
  • 13:40 - 13:42
    like savings and insurance,
    that are invisible.
  • 13:43 - 13:45
    And now the question is:
    How do we make them visible?
  • 13:46 - 13:50
    So let's go back to our rocket model.
  • 13:50 - 13:53
    We have to, first of all,
    look at the system
  • 13:53 - 13:56
    and see where there's little things
    we can fix, with friction,
  • 13:56 - 13:59
    where is there
    that we can remove friction?
  • 13:59 - 14:03
    And then the next thing we want to do
    is to think broadly about the system,
  • 14:03 - 14:06
    and say, what other motivations
    can we bring in?
  • 14:06 - 14:08
    And that's a much more difficult exercise,
  • 14:08 - 14:11
    and we don't always know
    what would work best.
  • 14:11 - 14:13
    Is it going to be money?
    Is it going to be loss aversion?
  • 14:13 - 14:15
    Is it going to be
    something that is visible?
  • 14:15 - 14:18
    We don't know, and we have
    to try different things.
  • 14:18 - 14:21
    We also have to realize that
    our intuition sometimes misleads us.
  • 14:21 - 14:25
    We don't always necessarily know
    what would work the best.
  • 14:25 - 14:27
    So if we think about this gap
  • 14:27 - 14:29
    between where we could be
    and where we are,
  • 14:29 - 14:33
    it's a really sad thing to see this gap
    and to think about it.
  • 14:33 - 14:35
    But the good news is,
    there's lots we can do.
  • 14:36 - 14:39
    Some of the changes are easy,
    some of the changes are more complex.
  • 14:39 - 14:42
    But if we'll attack each problem directly,
  • 14:42 - 14:45
    not by just providing
    more information to people
  • 14:45 - 14:47
    but trying to change the friction,
  • 14:47 - 14:48
    add motivation,
  • 14:48 - 14:50
    I think we can ...
  • 14:50 - 14:52
    Can we close the gap? No.
  • 14:52 - 14:55
    But can we get much better?
    Absolutely, yes.
  • 14:55 - 14:56
    Thank you very much.
  • 14:56 - 15:00
    (Applause)
Title:
How to change your behavior for the better
Speaker:
Dan Ariely
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
15:13

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions