Return to Video

9 myths about psychology: debunked

  • 0:01 - 0:04
    You've heard of your IQ,
    your general intelligence,
  • 0:04 - 0:06
    but what's your Psy-Q?
  • 0:06 - 0:08
    How much do you know
    about what makes you tick,
  • 0:08 - 0:11
    and how good are you
    at predicting other people's behavior
  • 0:11 - 0:12
    or even your own?
  • 0:12 - 0:15
    And how much of what you think you know
    about psychology is wrong?
  • 0:15 - 0:19
    Let's find out by counting down
    the top myths of psychology.
  • 0:19 - 0:22
    You've probably heard it said
    that when it comes to their psychology,
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    it's almost as if men are from Mars
    and women are from Venus.
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    But how different
    are men and women, really?
  • 0:28 - 0:30
    To find out, let's start
    by looking at something
  • 0:30 - 0:32
    on which men and women really do differ
  • 0:32 - 0:35
    and plotting some psychological
    gender differences on the same scale.
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    One thing men and women
    do really differ on
  • 0:37 - 0:39
    is how far they can throw a ball.
  • 0:39 - 0:41
    So if we look at the data for men here,
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    we see what is called
    a normal distribution curve.
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    A few men can throw a ball really far,
    a few men, not far at all,
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    but most, a kind of average distance.
  • 0:48 - 0:50
    And women share
    the same distribution as well,
  • 0:50 - 0:52
    but actually, there's
    quite a big difference.
  • 0:52 - 0:55
    In fact, the average man
    can throw a ball further
  • 0:55 - 0:57
    than about 98 percent of all women.
  • 0:57 - 1:00
    Now let's look at what some psychological
    gender differences look like
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    on the same standardized scale.
  • 1:03 - 1:04
    Any psychologist will tell you
  • 1:04 - 1:07
    that men are better
    at spatial awareness than women --
  • 1:07 - 1:09
    things like map-reading,
    for example -- and it's true.
  • 1:09 - 1:12
    But let's have a look
    at the size of this difference.
  • 1:12 - 1:15
    It's tiny; the lines are so close
    together, they almost overlap.
  • 1:15 - 1:19
    In fact, the average woman is better
    than 33 percent of all men,
  • 1:19 - 1:21
    and of course, if that was 50 percent,
  • 1:21 - 1:23
    then the two genders
    would be exactly equal.
  • 1:24 - 1:27
    It's worth bearing in mind that this
    difference and the next one I'll show you
  • 1:27 - 1:30
    are pretty much the biggest
    psychological gender differences
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    ever discovered in psychology.
  • 1:32 - 1:33
    Here's the next one.
  • 1:33 - 1:34
    Any psychologist will tell you
  • 1:34 - 1:37
    that women are better
    with language and grammar than men.
  • 1:37 - 1:39
    Here's performance
    on the standardized grammar test.
  • 1:39 - 1:41
    There, the women. There go the men.
  • 1:41 - 1:43
    Again, yes, women are better on average,
  • 1:43 - 1:44
    but the lines are so close
  • 1:44 - 1:48
    that 33 percent of men
    are better than the average woman.
  • 1:48 - 1:49
    And again, if it was 50 percent,
  • 1:49 - 1:52
    that would represent
    complete gender equality.
  • 1:52 - 1:54
    So it's not really
    a case of Mars and Venus.
  • 1:54 - 1:57
    It's more a case of, if anything,
    Mars and Snickers:
  • 1:57 - 2:01
    basically the same, but one's maybe
    slightly nuttier than the other.
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    When making a cake, do you prefer
    to use a recipe book with pictures?
  • 2:05 - 2:06
    Yeah, a few people.
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    Have a friend talk you through?
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    Or have a go, making it up
    as you go along?
  • 2:12 - 2:13
    Quite a few people there.
  • 2:13 - 2:15
    OK, so if you said A,
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    then this means
    that you're a visual learner,
  • 2:17 - 2:21
    and you learn best when information
    is presented in a visual style.
  • 2:21 - 2:23
    If you said B, it means
    you're an auditory learner,
  • 2:23 - 2:27
    that you learn best when information
    is presented to you in an auditory format.
  • 2:27 - 2:30
    And if you said C, it means
    that you're a kinesthetic learner,
  • 2:30 - 2:33
    that you learn best when you get stuck in
    and do things with your hands.
  • 2:33 - 2:36
    Except, of course,
    as you've probably guessed,
  • 2:36 - 2:38
    that it doesn't, because
    the whole thing is a complete myth.
  • 2:39 - 2:42
    Learning styles are made up and are not
    supported by scientific evidence.
  • 2:42 - 2:46
    We know this because in tightly
    controlled experimental studies
  • 2:46 - 2:48
    when learners are given material to learn,
  • 2:48 - 2:51
    either in their preferred style
    or an opposite style,
  • 2:51 - 2:54
    it makes no difference at all
    to the amount of information they retain.
  • 2:54 - 2:56
    And if you think about it
    for just a second,
  • 2:56 - 2:58
    it's obvious that this has to be true.
  • 2:58 - 3:03
    It's obvious that the best presentation
    format depends not on you,
  • 3:03 - 3:04
    but on what you're trying to learn.
  • 3:04 - 3:08
    Could you learn to drive a car,
    for example, just by listening to someone
  • 3:08 - 3:09
    telling you what to do,
  • 3:09 - 3:11
    with no kinesthetic experience?
  • 3:11 - 3:14
    Could you solve simultaneous equations
    by talking them through in your head,
  • 3:14 - 3:15
    without writing them down?
  • 3:15 - 3:19
    Could you revise for your architecture
    exams using interpretive dance
  • 3:19 - 3:20
    if you're a kinesthetic learner?
  • 3:20 - 3:23
    No; what you need to do is match
    the material to be learned
  • 3:23 - 3:24
    to the presentation format,
  • 3:25 - 3:26
    not you.
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    I know many of you are A-level students
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    that will have recently gotten
    your GCSE results.
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    And if you didn't quite get
    what you were hoping for,
  • 3:34 - 3:36
    then you can't really blame
    your learning style.
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    But one thing that you might want
    to think about blaming is your genes.
  • 3:40 - 3:43
    So what this is all about is that a recent
    study at University College London
  • 3:43 - 3:48
    found that 58 percent of the variation
    between different students
  • 3:48 - 3:49
    and their GCSE results
  • 3:49 - 3:51
    was down to genetic factors.
  • 3:51 - 3:54
    That sounds like a very precise figure.
    So how can we tell?
  • 3:54 - 3:57
    Well, when we want to unpack
    the relative contributions
  • 3:57 - 4:00
    of genes and the environment,
  • 4:00 - 4:02
    what we can do is a twin study.
  • 4:02 - 4:05
    Identical twins share
    100 percent of their environment
  • 4:05 - 4:07
    and 100 percent of their genes,
  • 4:07 - 4:11
    whereas nonidentical twins
    share 100 percent of their environment,
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    but just like any brother and sister,
    share only 50 percent of their genes.
  • 4:14 - 4:19
    So by comparing how similar
    GCSE results are in identical twins
  • 4:19 - 4:21
    versus nonidentical twins
  • 4:21 - 4:22
    and doing some clever maths,
  • 4:22 - 4:26
    we can get an idea of how much variation
    in performance is due to the environment,
  • 4:26 - 4:28
    and how much is due to genes.
  • 4:28 - 4:31
    And it turns out that it's about
    58 percent due to genes.
  • 4:32 - 4:36
    This isn't to undermine the hard work
    that you and your teachers here put in.
  • 4:36 - 4:39
    If you didn't quite get the GCSE results
    that you were hoping for,
  • 4:39 - 4:41
    then you can always try
    blaming your parents,
  • 4:41 - 4:43
    or at least their genes.
  • 4:43 - 4:45
    One thing that you shouldn't blame
  • 4:45 - 4:48
    is being a left-brained
    or right-brained learner,
  • 4:48 - 4:50
    because again, this is a myth.
  • 4:50 - 4:53
    The myth here is that
    the left brain is logical,
  • 4:53 - 4:55
    it's good with equations like this,
  • 4:55 - 4:58
    and the right brain is more creative,
    so the right brain is better at music.
  • 4:58 - 5:00
    But again, this is a myth,
  • 5:00 - 5:01
    because nearly everything you do
  • 5:01 - 5:04
    involves nearly all parts
    of your brain talking together,
  • 5:04 - 5:07
    even just the most mundane thing
    like having a normal conversation.
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    However, perhaps one reason
    why this myth has survived
  • 5:11 - 5:13
    is that there is
    a slight grain of truth to it.
  • 5:13 - 5:17
    A related version of the myth is that
    left-handed people are more creative
  • 5:17 - 5:18
    than right-handed people,
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    which kind of makes sense because
    your brain controls the opposite hand.
  • 5:21 - 5:25
    So in left-handed people, the right side
    of the brain is slightly more active
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    than the left side of the brain,
  • 5:27 - 5:30
    and the idea is the right-hand side
    is more creative.
  • 5:30 - 5:33
    Now, it isn't true per se
    that left-handed people are more creative
  • 5:33 - 5:34
    than right-handed people.
  • 5:34 - 5:36
    But what is true is that
    ambidextrous people,
  • 5:36 - 5:39
    or people who use both hands
    for different tasks,
  • 5:39 - 5:43
    are more creative thinkers
    than one-handed people,
  • 5:43 - 5:45
    because being ambidextrous involves
  • 5:45 - 5:47
    having both sides of the brain
    talk to each other a lot,
  • 5:47 - 5:51
    which seems to be involved
    in creative and flexible thinking.
  • 5:51 - 5:54
    The myth of the creative left-hander
    arises from the fact
  • 5:54 - 5:57
    that being ambidextrous
    is more common amongst left-handers
  • 5:57 - 5:58
    than right-handers,
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    so a grain of truth in the idea
    of the creative left-hander,
  • 6:01 - 6:02
    but not much.
  • 6:03 - 6:05
    A related myth that you've
    probably heard of
  • 6:05 - 6:07
    is that we only use
    10 percent of our brains.
  • 6:07 - 6:09
    This is, again, a complete myth.
  • 6:09 - 6:12
    Nearly everything that we do,
    even the most mundane thing,
  • 6:12 - 6:14
    uses nearly all of our brains.
  • 6:15 - 6:17
    That said, it is of course true
  • 6:17 - 6:22
    that most of us don't use our brainpower
    quite as well as we could.
  • 6:22 - 6:25
    So what could we do
    to boost our brainpower?
  • 6:25 - 6:27
    Maybe we could listen
    to a nice bit of Mozart.
  • 6:27 - 6:30
    Have you heard of the idea
    of the Mozart effect?
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    The idea is that listening
    to Mozart makes you smarter
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    and improves your performance on IQ tests.
  • 6:36 - 6:38
    Now again, what's interesting
    about this myth
  • 6:38 - 6:41
    is that although it's basically a myth,
    there is a grain of truth to it.
  • 6:41 - 6:43
    So the original study found that
  • 6:43 - 6:46
    participants who were played
    Mozart music for a few minutes
  • 6:46 - 6:48
    did better on a subsequent IQ test
  • 6:48 - 6:52
    than participants who simply
    sat in silence.
  • 6:52 - 6:56
    But a follow-up study recruited
    some people who liked Mozart music
  • 6:56 - 6:57
    and then another group of people
  • 6:57 - 7:00
    who were fans of
    the horror stories of Stephen King.
  • 7:00 - 7:03
    And they played the people
    the music or the stories.
  • 7:04 - 7:06
    The people who preferred
    Mozart music to the stories
  • 7:06 - 7:09
    got a bigger IQ boost
    from the Mozart than the stories,
  • 7:09 - 7:12
    but the people who preferred
    the stories to the Mozart music
  • 7:12 - 7:15
    got a bigger IQ boost
    from listening to the Stephen King stories
  • 7:15 - 7:16
    than the Mozart music.
  • 7:16 - 7:19
    So the truth is that listening
    to something that you enjoy
  • 7:19 - 7:22
    perks you up a bit
    and gives you a temporary IQ boost
  • 7:22 - 7:24
    on a narrow range of tasks.
  • 7:24 - 7:26
    There's no suggestion that
    listening to Mozart,
  • 7:26 - 7:28
    or indeed Stephen King stories,
  • 7:28 - 7:31
    is going to make you any smarter
    in the long run.
  • 7:32 - 7:34
    Another version of the Mozart myth
  • 7:34 - 7:39
    is that listening to Mozart can make you
    not only cleverer but healthier, too.
  • 7:40 - 7:42
    Unfortunately, this doesn't
    seem to be true
  • 7:42 - 7:45
    of someone who listened
    to the music of Mozart almost every day,
  • 7:45 - 7:46
    Mozart himself,
  • 7:46 - 7:49
    who suffered from gonorrhea,
    smallpox, arthritis,
  • 7:49 - 7:53
    and, what most people think eventually
    killed him in the end, syphilis.
  • 7:54 - 7:57
    This suggests that Mozart should have been
    a bit more careful, perhaps,
  • 7:57 - 7:59
    when choosing his sexual partners.
  • 7:59 - 8:01
    But how do we choose a partner?
  • 8:01 - 8:07
    So a myth that I have to say
    is sometimes spread a bit by sociologists
  • 8:07 - 8:10
    is that our preferences in a romantic
    partner are a product of our culture,
  • 8:10 - 8:12
    that they're very culturally specific.
  • 8:12 - 8:14
    But in fact, the data don't back this up.
  • 8:14 - 8:18
    A famous study surveyed people from
    [37] different cultures across the globe
  • 8:18 - 8:20
    from Americans to Zulus,
  • 8:20 - 8:22
    on what they look for in a partner.
  • 8:22 - 8:24
    And in every single culture
    across the globe,
  • 8:24 - 8:28
    men placed more value
    on physical attractiveness in a partner
  • 8:28 - 8:30
    than did women,
  • 8:30 - 8:31
    and in every single culture, too,
  • 8:31 - 8:36
    women placed more importance than did men
    on ambition and high earning power.
  • 8:36 - 8:37
    In every culture, too,
  • 8:37 - 8:40
    men preferred women
    who were younger than themselves,
  • 8:40 - 8:43
    an average of, I think it was 2.66 years.
  • 8:43 - 8:44
    And in every culture, too,
  • 8:44 - 8:47
    women preferred men
    who were older than them,
  • 8:47 - 8:50
    so an average of 3.42 years,
  • 8:50 - 8:53
    which is why we've got here,
    "Everybody needs a Sugar Daddy."
  • 8:53 - 8:54
    (Laughter)
  • 8:54 - 8:56
    So moving on from trying
    to score with a partner
  • 8:56 - 9:00
    to trying to score in basketball
    or football or whatever your sport is.
  • 9:00 - 9:04
    The myth here is that sportsmen go through
    "hot hand" streaks, Americans call them,
  • 9:04 - 9:06
    or "purple patches,"
    we sometimes say in England,
  • 9:06 - 9:09
    where they just can't miss,
    like this guy here.
  • 9:09 - 9:13
    But in fact, what happens is that
    if you analyze the pattern
  • 9:13 - 9:14
    of hits and misses statistically,
  • 9:14 - 9:17
    it turns out that it's
    nearly always at random.
  • 9:17 - 9:19
    Your brain creates patterns
    from the randomness.
  • 9:19 - 9:21
    If you toss a coin,
  • 9:21 - 9:25
    a streak of heads or tails is going
    to come out somewhere in the randomness,
  • 9:25 - 9:28
    and because the brain likes to see
    patterns where there are none,
  • 9:28 - 9:30
    we look at these streaks
    and attribute meaning to them
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    and say, "Yeah he's really on form today,"
  • 9:32 - 9:35
    whereas actually you would
    get the same pattern
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    if you were just getting
    hits and misses at random.
  • 9:39 - 9:42
    An exception to this, however,
    is penalty shootouts.
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    A recent study looking at
    penalty shootouts in football
  • 9:45 - 9:47
    showed that players
    who represent countries
  • 9:47 - 9:49
    with a very bad record
    in penalty shootouts,
  • 9:49 - 9:51
    like, for example, England,
  • 9:51 - 9:55
    tend to be quicker to take their shots
    than countries with a better record,
  • 9:55 - 9:59
    and presumably as a result,
    they're more likely to miss.
  • 9:59 - 10:01
    Which raises the question
  • 10:01 - 10:04
    of if there's any way we could improve
    people's performance.
  • 10:04 - 10:06
    And one thing you might think about doing
  • 10:06 - 10:09
    is punishing people for their misses
    and seeing if that improves them.
  • 10:09 - 10:13
    This idea, the effect that punishment
    can improve performance,
  • 10:13 - 10:15
    was what participants
    thought they were testing
  • 10:15 - 10:18
    in Milgram's famous learning
    and punishment experiment
  • 10:18 - 10:21
    that you've probably heard about
    if you're a psychology student.
  • 10:21 - 10:23
    The story goes that participants
    were prepared to give
  • 10:23 - 10:27
    what they believed to be fatal
    electric shocks to a fellow participant
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    when they got a question wrong,
  • 10:29 - 10:31
    just because someone
    in a white coat told them to.
  • 10:31 - 10:34
    But this story is a myth
    for three reasons.
  • 10:34 - 10:39
    Firstly, and most crucially, the lab coat
    wasn't white. It was, in fact, grey.
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    Secondly, the participants
    were told before the study
  • 10:43 - 10:46
    and reminded any time
    they raised a concern,
  • 10:46 - 10:48
    that although the shocks were painful,
    they were not fatal
  • 10:48 - 10:51
    and indeed caused
    no permanent damage whatsoever.
  • 10:51 - 10:54
    And thirdly, participants
    didn't give the shocks
  • 10:54 - 10:57
    just because someone
    in the coat told them to.
  • 10:57 - 10:59
    When they were interviewed
    after the study,
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    all the participants said
    that they firmly believed
  • 11:01 - 11:05
    that the learning and punishment study
    served a worthy scientific purpose
  • 11:05 - 11:07
    which would have
    enduring gains for science,
  • 11:07 - 11:13
    as opposed to the momentary, nonfatal
    discomfort caused to the participants.
  • 11:14 - 11:17
    OK, so I've been talking
    for about 12 minutes now,
  • 11:17 - 11:19
    and you've probably been
    sitting there listening to me,
  • 11:19 - 11:22
    analyzing my speech patterns
    and body language
  • 11:22 - 11:25
    and trying to work out if you should
    take any notice of what I'm saying,
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    whether I'm telling the truth
    or whether I'm lying.
  • 11:28 - 11:30
    But if so, you've probably
    completely failed,
  • 11:30 - 11:32
    because although we all think
    we can catch a liar
  • 11:32 - 11:34
    from their body language
    and speech patterns,
  • 11:34 - 11:38
    hundreds of psychological tests
    over the years have shown that all of us,
  • 11:38 - 11:40
    including police officers and detectives,
  • 11:40 - 11:43
    are basically at chance when it comes
    to detecting lies from body language
  • 11:43 - 11:44
    and verbal patterns.
  • 11:44 - 11:46
    Interestingly, there is one exception:
  • 11:46 - 11:48
    TV appeals for missing relatives.
  • 11:49 - 11:52
    It's quite easy to predict
    when the relatives are missing
  • 11:52 - 11:55
    and when the appealers have, in fact,
    murdered the relatives themselves.
  • 11:55 - 11:59
    So hoax appealers are more likely
    to shake their heads, to look away,
  • 11:59 - 12:00
    and to make errors in their speech,
  • 12:00 - 12:03
    whereas genuine appealers
    are more likely to express hope
  • 12:03 - 12:05
    that the person will return safely
  • 12:05 - 12:06
    and to avoid brutal language.
  • 12:06 - 12:11
    So, for example, they might say
    "taken from us" rather than "killed."
  • 12:11 - 12:14
    Speaking of which,
    it's about time I killed this talk,
  • 12:14 - 12:17
    but before I do, I just want
    to give you, in 30 seconds,
  • 12:17 - 12:20
    the overarching myth of psychology.
  • 12:20 - 12:25
    The myth is that psychology is just
    a collection of interesting theories,
  • 12:25 - 12:28
    all of which say something useful
    and all of which have something to offer.
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    What I hope to have shown you
    in the past few minutes
  • 12:31 - 12:32
    is that this isn't true.
  • 12:32 - 12:35
    What we need to do is assess
    psychological theories
  • 12:36 - 12:37
    by seeing what predictions they make,
  • 12:37 - 12:40
    whether that is that listening to Mozart
    makes you smarter,
  • 12:40 - 12:45
    that you learn better when information is
    presented in your preferred learning style
  • 12:45 - 12:46
    or whatever it is,
  • 12:46 - 12:48
    all of these are testable
    empirical predictions,
  • 12:48 - 12:50
    and the only way we can make progress
  • 12:50 - 12:52
    is to test these predictions
    against the data
  • 12:52 - 12:55
    in tightly controlled
    experimental studies.
  • 12:55 - 12:57
    And it's only by doing so
    that we can hope to discover
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    which of these theories
    are well supported,
  • 13:00 - 13:04
    and which, like all the ones
    I've told you about today, are myths.
  • 13:04 - 13:05
    Thank you.
  • 13:05 - 13:08
    (Applause)
Title:
9 myths about psychology: debunked
Speaker:
Ben Ambridge
Description:

How much of what you think about psychology is actually wrong? In this whistle-stop tour of disproved ideas, Ben Ambridge shares nine popular ideas about psychology that have been proven wrong -- and uncovers a few surprising truths about how our brains really work.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDTalks
Duration:
14:55

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions