Return to Video

Why people fall for misinformation - Joseph Isaac

  • 0:07 - 0:10
    In 1901, David Hänig published a paper
  • 0:10 - 0:14
    that forever changed
    our understanding of taste.
  • 0:14 - 0:18
    His research led to what we know
    today as the taste map:
  • 0:18 - 0:22
    an illustration that divides the tongue
    into four separate areas.
  • 0:22 - 0:24
    According to this map,
  • 0:24 - 0:27
    receptors at the tip of our tongues
    capture sweetness,
  • 0:27 - 0:30
    bitterness
    is detected at the tongue’s base,
  • 0:30 - 0:35
    and along the sides, receptors capture
    salty and sour sensations.
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    Since its invention, the taste map
    has been published
  • 0:38 - 0:40
    in textbooks and newspapers.
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    The only problem with this map,
    is that it’s wrong.
  • 0:44 - 0:47
    In fact, it’s not even
    an accurate representation
  • 0:47 - 0:50
    of what Hänig originally discovered.
  • 0:50 - 0:53
    The tongue map is a common misconception—
  • 0:53 - 0:56
    something widely believed
    but largely incorrect.
  • 0:56 - 0:59
    So where do misconceptions
    like this come from,
  • 0:59 - 1:03
    and what makes a fake fact
    so easy to believe?
  • 1:03 - 1:07
    It’s true that the tongue map’s
    journey begins with David Hänig.
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    As part of his dissertation
    at Leipzig University,
  • 1:10 - 1:16
    Hänig analyzed taste sensitivities across
    the tongue for the four basic flavors.
  • 1:16 - 1:20
    Using sucrose for sweet,
    quinine sulfate for bitter,
  • 1:20 - 1:24
    hydrochloric acid for sour,
    and salt for salty,
  • 1:24 - 1:29
    Hänig applied these stimuli to compare
    differences in taste thresholds
  • 1:29 - 1:31
    across a subject’s tongue.
  • 1:31 - 1:34
    He hoped to better understand
    the physiological mechanisms
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    that affected these four flavors,
  • 1:36 - 1:40
    and his data suggested that sensitivity
    for each taste
  • 1:40 - 1:43
    did in fact vary across the tongue.
  • 1:43 - 1:47
    The maximum sensation for sweet
    was located at the tongue’s tip;
  • 1:47 - 1:52
    bitter flavors were strongest at the back;
    salt was strongest in this area,
  • 1:52 - 1:55
    and sour at the middle
    of the tongue’s sides.
  • 1:55 - 1:58
    But Hänig was careful
    to note that every sensation
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    could also be tasted across the tongue,
  • 2:01 - 2:07
    and that the areas he identified offered
    very small variations in intensity.
  • 2:07 - 2:09
    Like so many misconceptions,
  • 2:09 - 2:13
    the tongue map represents a distortion
    of its original source,
  • 2:13 - 2:16
    however the nature of that distortion
    can vary.
  • 2:16 - 2:19
    Some misconceptions
    are comprised of disinformation—
  • 2:19 - 2:23
    false information intentionally designed
    to mislead people.
  • 2:23 - 2:26
    But many misconceptions,
    including the tongue map,
  • 2:26 - 2:30
    center on misinformation—
    false or misleading information
  • 2:30 - 2:35
    that results from unintentional
    inaccuracy.
  • 2:35 - 2:39
    Misinformation is most often shaped
    by mistakes and human error,
  • 2:39 - 2:42
    but the specific mistakes
    that lead to a misconception
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    can be surprisingly varied.
  • 2:45 - 2:46
    In the case of the tongue map,
  • 2:46 - 2:49
    Hänig’s dissertation was written
    in German,
  • 2:49 - 2:53
    meaning the paper could only be
    understood by readers fluent in German
  • 2:53 - 2:57
    and well versed in Hanig’s
    small corner of academia.
  • 2:57 - 3:02
    This kicked off a game of telephone
    that re-shaped Häing’s research
  • 3:02 - 3:05
    every time it was shared
    with outside parties.
  • 3:05 - 3:08
    Less than a decade after his dissertation,
  • 3:08 - 3:11
    newspapers were falsely insisting
    that experiments
  • 3:11 - 3:16
    could prove sweetness was imperceptible
    on the back of the tongue.
  • 3:16 - 3:19
    The second culprit
    behind the tongue map’s spread
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    were the images
    that Hänig’s work inspired.
  • 3:22 - 3:27
    In 1912, a rough version of the map
    appeared in a newspaper article
  • 3:27 - 3:29
    that cautiously described
    some of the mysteries
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    behind taste and smell research.
  • 3:32 - 3:36
    Featuring clear labels across the tongue,
    the article’s illustration
  • 3:36 - 3:40
    simplified Hänig’s more-complicated
    original diagrams.
  • 3:40 - 3:45
    Variations of this approachable image
    became repeatedly cited,
  • 3:45 - 3:49
    often without credit or nuanced
    consideration for Hänig’s work.
  • 3:49 - 3:53
    Eventually this image spread
    to textbooks and classrooms
  • 3:53 - 3:58
    as a purported truth
    of how we experience taste.
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    But perhaps the factor that most
    contributed to this misconception
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    was its narrative simplicity.
  • 4:04 - 4:05
    In many ways,
  • 4:05 - 4:10
    the map complements our desire for clear
    stories about the world around us—
  • 4:10 - 4:15
    a quality not always present
    in the sometimes-messy fields of science.
  • 4:15 - 4:16
    For example,
  • 4:16 - 4:21
    even the number of tastes we have is more
    complicated than Hänig’s work suggests.
  • 4:21 - 4:27
    Umami— also known as savory—
    is now considered the fifth basic taste,
  • 4:27 - 4:30
    and many still debate
    the existence of tastes
  • 4:30 - 4:34
    like fatty, alkaline, metallic,
    and water-like.
  • 4:34 - 4:36
    Once we hear a good story,
  • 4:36 - 4:40
    it can be difficult to change
    how we see that information,
  • 4:40 - 4:42
    even in the face of new evidence.
  • 4:42 - 4:47
    So, next time you see a convenient chart
    or read a surprising anecdote,
  • 4:47 - 4:50
    try to maintain a healthy skepticism—
  • 4:50 - 4:53
    because misconceptions
    can leave a bitter taste
  • 4:53 - 4:55
    on every part of your tongue.
Title:
Why people fall for misinformation - Joseph Isaac
Speaker:
Joseph Isaac
Description:

View full lesson: https://ed.ted.com/lessons/why-people-fall-for-misinformation-joseph-isaac

In 1901, David Hänig published research that led to what we know today as the taste map: an illustration that divides the tongue into four separate areas. It has since been published in textbooks and newspapers. There is just one problem: the map is wrong. So how do misconceptions like this spread, and what makes a fake fact so easy to believe? Joseph Isaac dives into the world of misinformation.

Lesson by Joseph Isaac, directed by CUB Animation.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:57
Elise Haadsma approved English subtitles for Why people fall for misinformation
Elise Haadsma accepted English subtitles for Why people fall for misinformation
lauren mcalpine edited English subtitles for Why people fall for misinformation

English subtitles

Revisions